Intentional Minds: A Philosophical Analysis of Intention Tested through fMRI Experiments Involving People with Schizophrenia, People with Autism, and Healthy Individuals

In this paper we show how we empirically tested one of the most relevant topics in philosophy of mind through a series of fMRI experiments: the classification of different types of intention. To this aim, firstly we trace a theoretical distinction among private, prospective, and communicative intentions. Second, we propose a set of predictions concerning the recognition of these three types of intention in healthy individuals, and we report the experimental results corroborating our theoretical model of intention. Third, we derive from our model predictions relevant for the domain of psychopathological functioning. In particular, we treat the cases of both hyper-intentionality (as in paranoid schizophrenia) and hypo-intentionality (as in autistic spectrum disorders). Our conclusion is that the theoretical model of intention we propose contributes to enlarge our knowledge on the neurobiological bases of intention processing, in both healthy people and in people with impairments to the neurocognitive system that underlies intention recognition.

[1]  M. Coltheart,et al.  Recognition of metaphor and irony in young adults: the impact of schizotypal personality traits , 2004, Psychiatry Research.

[2]  M. Tomasello Origins of human communication , 2008 .

[3]  J. Piven,et al.  Visual Scanning of Faces in Autism , 2002, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[4]  U. Frith Mind Blindness and the Brain in Autism , 2001, Neuron.

[5]  R. Hari,et al.  Brain basis of human social interaction: from concepts to brain imaging. , 2009, Physiological reviews.

[6]  John McGregor,et al.  Intentionality , 2010, Journal of Object Technology.

[7]  J. Burns,et al.  Psychosis: A costly by-product of social brain evolution in Homo sapiens , 2006, Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry.

[8]  Ellen Vos,et al.  Theory of mind in schizophrenia: meta-analysis. , 2007, The British journal of psychiatry : the journal of mental science.

[9]  J. Decety,et al.  The detection of intentional contingencies in simple animations in patients with delusions of persecution , 2003, Psychological Medicine.

[10]  P. Skudlarski,et al.  The role of the fusiform face area in social cognition: implications for the pathobiology of autism. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[11]  M. Tamietto,et al.  Fast recognition of social emotions takes the whole brain: Interhemispheric cooperation in the absence of cerebral asymmetry , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[12]  Michael E. Bratman,et al.  Intention, Plans, and Practical Reason , 1991 .

[13]  B. Bara Cognitive pragmatics: The mental processes of communication , 2010 .

[14]  Bruno G. Bara,et al.  Intention Processing in Communication: A Common Brain Network for Language and Gestures , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[15]  F. Garbarini,et al.  The As If in Cognitive Science, Neuroscience and Anthropology , 2006 .

[16]  B. Crespi,et al.  Psychosis and autism as diametrical disorders of the social brain. , 2008, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[17]  D. Premack,et al.  Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? , 1978, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[18]  F. Volkmar,et al.  The enactive mind, or from actions to cognition: lessons from autism. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[19]  E. Fombonne,et al.  The epidemiology of autism: a review , 1999, Psychological Medicine.

[20]  C. Frith The social brain? , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[21]  J. Decety,et al.  Abnormalities of brain function during a nonverbal theory of mind task in schizophrenia , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[22]  F. Volkmar,et al.  Abnormal ventral temporal cortical activity during face discrimination among individuals with autism and Asperger syndrome. , 2000, Archives of general psychiatry.

[23]  S. Bölte,et al.  Functional imbalance of visual pathways indicates alternative face processing strategies in autism , 2003, Neurology.

[24]  D. Povinelli,et al.  Mindblindness. An Essay on Autism and Theory of Mind Simon Baron-Cohen 1995 , 1996, Trends in Neurosciences.

[25]  Robin I. M. Dunbar Social Brain Hypothesis , 1998, Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science.

[26]  A. Bailey,et al.  The possibility of different forms of theory of mind impairment in psychiatric and developmental disorders. , 2000, Psychological medicine.

[27]  Siobhan Chapman Logic and Conversation , 2005 .

[28]  Andrew Brook Introduction: Philosophy in and Philosophy of Cognitive Science , 2009, Top. Cogn. Sci..

[29]  D. Perrett,et al.  Imitation, mirror neurons and autism , 2001, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[30]  Ivan Enrici,et al.  Theory of mind ability in the behavioural variant of frontotemporal dementia: An analysis of the neural, cognitive, and social levels , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[31]  T. Tényi,et al.  Schizophrenics Show a Failure in the Decoding of Violations of Conversational Implicatures , 2002, Psychopathology.

[32]  F. Van Overwalle Social cognition and the brain: A meta‐analysis , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[33]  J. Fuster Frontal lobes , 1993, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[34]  R. Bentall,et al.  Persecutory delusions: a review and theoretical integration. , 2001, Clinical psychology review.

[35]  J. Burns An evolutionary theory of schizophrenia: Cortical connectivity, metarepresentation, and the social brain , 2004, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[36]  M. Brüne,et al.  Theory of mind—evolution, ontogeny, brain mechanisms and psychopathology , 2006, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[37]  C. Frith,et al.  Functional imaging of ‘theory of mind’ , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[38]  M Coltheart,et al.  Disturbed communication in schizophrenia: the role of poor pragmatics and poor mind-reading , 2002, Psychological Medicine.

[39]  E. Bullmore,et al.  Social intelligence in the normal and autistic brain: an fMRI study , 1999, The European journal of neuroscience.

[40]  Pascal Belin,et al.  Abnormal cortical voice processing in autism , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[41]  J. Decety,et al.  Social brain dysfunctions in schizophrenia: A review of neuroimaging studies , 2006, Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging.

[42]  M. Iacoboni,et al.  The mirror neuron system and the consequences of its dysfunction , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[43]  R Saxe,et al.  People thinking about thinking people The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind” , 2003, NeuroImage.

[44]  J. Decety,et al.  What you believe versus what you think they believe: a neuroimaging study of conceptual perspective‐taking , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.

[45]  Henrik Walter,et al.  Dysfunction of the social brain in schizophrenia is modulated by intention type: an fMRI study. , 2009, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[46]  C. Frith,et al.  Exploring ‘theory of mind’ in people with schizophrenia , 1996, Psychological Medicine.

[47]  R. Adolphs Investigating the cognitive neuroscience of social behavior , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[48]  M. Brüne Emotion recognition, ‘theory of mind,’ and social behavior in schizophrenia , 2005, Psychiatry Research.

[49]  E. Bora,et al.  Theory of mind impairment in schizophrenia: Meta-analysis , 2009, Schizophrenia Research.

[50]  Patrick Haggard,et al.  What are intentions , 2010 .

[51]  G. V. Van Hoesen,et al.  Prefrontal cortex in humans and apes: a comparative study of area 10. , 2001, American journal of physical anthropology.

[52]  Elisabeth Patherie,et al.  The Content of Intentions , 2000 .

[53]  J. McGrath,et al.  The cognitive neuropsychology of schizophrenia. , 1997 .

[54]  C. Frith,et al.  Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[55]  A. Bailey,et al.  Are there theory of mind regions in the brain? A review of the neuroimaging literature , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[56]  C. Frith,et al.  Schizophrenia and theory of mind , 2004, Psychological Medicine.

[57]  H. Critchley,et al.  The functional neuroanatomy of social behaviour: changes in cerebral blood flow when people with autistic disorder process facial expressions. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[58]  C. Frith,et al.  Autism, Asperger syndrome and brain mechanisms for the attribution of mental states to animated shapes. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[59]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The functional role of the parieto-frontal mirror circuit: interpretations and misinterpretations , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[60]  Roel M. Willems,et al.  Complementary Systems for Understanding Action Intentions , 2008, Current Biology.

[61]  A. Hamilton Goals, intentions and mental states: challenges for theories of autism. , 2009, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[62]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Other minds in the brain: a functional imaging study of “theory of mind” in story comprehension , 1995, Cognition.

[63]  Henrik Walter,et al.  Understanding Intentions in Social Interaction: The Role of the Anterior Paracingulate Cortex , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[64]  Robin I. M. Dunbar,et al.  The social brain hypothesis and its implications for social evolution , 2009, Annals of human biology.

[65]  E. Pacherie Towards a dynamic theory of intentions , 2009 .

[66]  Kim M. Dalton,et al.  Gaze fixation and the neural circuitry of face processing in autism , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[67]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  Frontal and Temporo-Parietal Lobe Contributions to Theory of Mind: Neuropsychological Evidence from a False-Belief Task with Reduced Language and Executive Demands , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[68]  J. Decety,et al.  A PET Investigation of the Attribution of Intentions with a Nonverbal Task , 2000, NeuroImage.

[69]  A. Goldman To Appear in: , 2008 .

[70]  Nicholas Humphrey,et al.  Consciousness Regained: Chapters in the Development of Mind , 1983 .

[71]  V. Ramachandran,et al.  Broken mirrors: a theory of autism. , 2006, Scientific American.

[72]  Aaron C. Koralek,et al.  Two Takes on the Social Brain: A Comparison of Theory of Mind Tasks , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[73]  H. Walter,et al.  The intentional network: How the brain reads varieties of intentions , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[74]  R. Adolphs Conceptual Challenges and Directions for Social Neuroscience , 2010, Neuron.

[75]  Cristina Becchio,et al.  How the brain understands intention: Different neural circuits identify the componential features of motor and prior intentions , 2006, Consciousness and Cognition.

[76]  R. Siegert,et al.  Theory of mind in schizophrenia: A critical review , 2005, Cognitive neuropsychiatry.

[77]  C. Frith,et al.  The appreciation of visual jokes in people with schizophrenia: a study of ‘mentalizing’ ability , 1997, Schizophrenia Research.

[78]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Intention Understanding in Autism , 2009, PloS one.

[79]  Kevin S. LaBar,et al.  Component Neural Systems for the Creation of Emotional Memories during Free Viewing of a Complex, Real-World Event , 2010, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[80]  U. Bonuccelli,et al.  Theory of Mind in Parkinson's disease , 2011, Behavioural Brain Research.

[81]  Uta Frith,et al.  Theory of mind , 2001, Current Biology.

[82]  Nathalie Boddaert,et al.  Autism, the superior temporal sulcus and social perception , 2006, Trends in Neurosciences.