Different impacts of long-term abacus training on symbolic and non-symbolic numerical magnitude processing in children

[1]  Feiyan Chen,et al.  Adaptive Reconfiguration of Intrinsic Community Structure in Children with 5-Year Abacus Training. , 2021, Cerebral cortex.

[2]  Gwangsun Kim,et al.  Visual number sense in untrained deep neural networks , 2021, Science Advances.

[3]  E. Brannon,et al.  Failure to replicate the benefit of approximate arithmetic training for symbolic arithmetic fluency in adults , 2020, Cognition.

[4]  Li Yuan,et al.  Visual form perception predicts 3-year longitudinal development of mathematical achievement , 2020, Cognitive Processing.

[5]  Alain Content,et al.  The neural signature of numerosity by separating numerical and continuous magnitude extraction in visual cortex with frequency-tagged EEG , 2019, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[6]  Yiyun Zhang,et al.  Visual form perception is fundamental for both reading comprehension and arithmetic computation , 2019, Cognition.

[7]  Jian Weng,et al.  The impact of long-term abacus training on modular properties of functional brain network , 2018, NeuroImage.

[8]  A. Proverbio,et al.  Distinct neural mechanisms for reading Arabic vs. verbal numbers: An ERP study , 2018, The European journal of neuroscience.

[9]  Xinlin Zhou,et al.  Dyslexia and dyscalculia are characterized by common visual perception deficits , 2018, Developmental neuropsychology.

[10]  Feiyan Chen,et al.  The Effects of Long-term Abacus Training on Topological Properties of Brain Functional Networks , 2017, Scientific Reports.

[11]  Ping Yang,et al.  The effects of changes in object location on object identity detection: A simultaneous EEG-fMRI study , 2017, NeuroImage.

[12]  Elizabeth M. Brannon,et al.  Numerosity processing in early visual cortex , 2017, NeuroImage.

[13]  Shanshan Dong,et al.  Effect of abacus training on executive function development and underlying neural correlates in Chinese children , 2017, Human brain mapping.

[14]  Feiyan Chen,et al.  The impact of abacus training on working memory and underlying neural correlates in young adults , 2016, Neuroscience.

[15]  A. Henik,et al.  The contribution of fish studies to the “number sense” debate , 2016, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[16]  David Barner,et al.  Learning Mathematics in a Visuospatial Format: A Randomized, Controlled Trial of Mental Abacus Instruction. , 2016, Child development.

[17]  Joonkoo Park,et al.  Rapid and Direct Encoding of Numerosity in the Visual Stream. , 2015, Cerebral cortex.

[18]  A. González-Garrido,et al.  Comparisons of numerical magnitudes in children with different levels of mathematical achievement. An ERP study , 2015, Brain Research.

[19]  Y. Yao,et al.  Abacus Training Affects Math and Task Switching Abilities and Modulates Their Relationships in Chinese Children , 2015, PloS one.

[20]  Yiyun Zhang,et al.  Visual perception can account for the close relation between numerosity processing and computational fluency , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[21]  Fenglei Du,et al.  Numerical processing efficiency improved in children using mental abacus: ERP evidence utilizing a numerical Stroop task , 2015, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[22]  Guido Marco Cicchini,et al.  Mechanisms for perception of numerosity or texture-density are governed by crowding-like effects. , 2015, Journal of vision.

[23]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  Qualitatively different coding of symbolic and nonsymbolic numbers in the human brain , 2015, Human brain mapping.

[24]  Yuan Yao,et al.  Long-Term Abacus Training Induces Automatic Processing of Abacus Numbers in Children , 2014, Perception.

[25]  Daniel C. Hyde,et al.  Brief non-symbolic, approximate number practice enhances subsequent exact symbolic arithmetic in children , 2014, Cognition.

[26]  J. Bulthé,et al.  Format-dependent representations of symbolic and non-symbolic numbers in the human cortex as revealed by multi-voxel pattern analyses , 2014, NeuroImage.

[27]  G. A. Miller,et al.  Committee report: publication guidelines and recommendations for studies using electroencephalography and magnetoencephalography. , 2014, Psychophysiology.

[28]  Hong Zhang,et al.  Abacus Training Modulates the Neural Correlates of Exact and Approximate Calculations in Chinese Children: An fMRI Study , 2013, BioMed research international.

[29]  M. Just,et al.  Decoding the representation of numerical values from brain activation patterns , 2013, Human brain mapping.

[30]  A. Jacobs,et al.  An electrophysiological investigation of non-symbolic magnitude processing: Numerical distance effects in children with and without mathematical learning disabilities , 2013, Cortex.

[31]  Elizabeth M Brannon,et al.  Training the Approximate Number System Improves Math Proficiency , 2013, Psychological science.

[32]  Justin Halberda,et al.  Is Approximate Number Precision a Stable Predictor of Math Ability? , 2013, Learning and individual differences.

[33]  Feiyan Chen,et al.  Numerical processing efficiency improved in experienced mental abacus children , 2013, Cognition.

[34]  Stella F. Lourenco,et al.  The approximate number system and its relation to early math achievement: evidence from the preschool years. , 2013, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[35]  Gastone G. Celesia,et al.  Visual evoked potentials and electroretinograms , 2012 .

[36]  David Barner,et al.  Representing exact number visually using mental abacus. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[37]  U. Goswami,et al.  Executive function effects and numerical development in children : behavioural and ERP evidence from a numerical Stroop paradigm , 2011 .

[38]  Sian L. Beilock,et al.  Numerical ordering ability mediates the relation between number-sense and arithmetic competence , 2011, Cognition.

[39]  Melissa E. Libertus,et al.  Preschool acuity of the approximate number system correlates with school math ability. , 2011, Developmental science.

[40]  S. Varma,et al.  Dyscalculia: From Brain to Education , 2011, Science.

[41]  Fengji Geng,et al.  Enhanced white matter tracts integrity in children with abacus training , 2011, Human brain mapping.

[42]  Andrea Facoetti,et al.  Developmental trajectory of number acuity reveals a severe impairment in developmental dyscalculia , 2010, Cognition.

[43]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  How is phonological processing related to individual differences in children's arithmetic skills? , 2010, Developmental science.

[44]  Marie-Pascale Noël,et al.  Symbolic and nonsymbolic number comparison in children with and without dyscalculia , 2010, Cognition.

[45]  Gui Xue,et al.  Effects of symbol type and numerical distance on the human event-related potential , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[46]  Fruzsina Soltész,et al.  Relationships between magnitude representation, counting and memory in 4- to 7-year-old children: A developmental study , 2010, Behavioral and Brain Functions.

[47]  Inkeri K. Herfs,et al.  The development of automated access to symbolic and non‐symbolic number knowledge in children: an ERP study , 2009, The European journal of neuroscience.

[48]  L. Verschaffel,et al.  The predictive value of numerical magnitude comparison for individual differences in mathematics achievement. , 2009, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[49]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  Mapping numerical magnitudes onto symbols: the numerical distance effect and individual differences in children's mathematics achievement. , 2009, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[50]  David Barner,et al.  Finding one’s meaning: A test of the relation between quantifiers and integers in language development , 2009, Cognitive Psychology.

[51]  Justin Halberda,et al.  Individual differences in non-verbal number acuity correlate with maths achievement , 2008, Nature.

[52]  M. Noël,et al.  Does finger training increase young children's numerical performance? , 2008, Cortex.

[53]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Distinct Cerebral Pathways for Object Identity and Number in Human Infants , 2008, PLoS biology.

[54]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Cultural Recycling of Cortical Maps , 2007, Neuron.

[55]  V. Csépe,et al.  The speed of magnitude processing and executive functions in controlled and automatic number comparison in children: an electro-encephalography study , 2007, Behavioral and Brain Functions.

[56]  Christophe Mussolin,et al.  The nonintentional processing of Arabic numbers in children , 2007, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[57]  M. Noël,et al.  Basic numerical skills in children with mathematics learning disabilities: A comparison of symbolic vs non-symbolic number magnitude processing , 2007, Cognition.

[58]  S. Dehaene,et al.  A Magnitude Code Common to Numerosities and Number Symbols in Human Intraparietal Cortex , 2007, Neuron.

[59]  Melissa E. Libertus,et al.  Electrophysiological evidence for notation independence in numerical processing , 2007, Behavioral and Brain Functions.

[60]  Zhenghui Hu,et al.  Neural correlates of serial abacus mental calculation in children: A functional MRI study , 2006, Neuroscience Letters.

[61]  Florian Koppelstaetter,et al.  Neural correlates of the number–size interference task in children , 2006, Neuroreport.

[62]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  Neural correlates of symbolic number processing in children and adults , 2005, Neuroreport.

[63]  V Menon,et al.  Cerebral Cortex doi:10.1093/cercor/bhi055 Developmental Changes in Mental Arithmetic: Evidence for Increased Functional Specialization in the Left Inferior Parietal Cortex , 2005 .

[64]  Daniel Brandeis,et al.  Emerging Neurophysiological Specialization for Letter Strings , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[65]  Charles Hulme,et al.  The cognitive foundations of reading and arithmetic skills in 7- to 10-year-olds. , 2005, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[66]  Wim Fias,et al.  Representation of Number in Animals and Humans: A Neural Model , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[67]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Exact and Approximate Arithmetic in an Amazonian Indigene Group , 2004, Science.

[68]  Bruno Rossion,et al.  Electrophysiological evidence for differential processing of numerical quantity and order in humans. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[69]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  Developmental dyscalculia and basic numerical capacities: a study of 8–9-year-old students , 2004, Cognition.

[70]  E. Spelke,et al.  Language and Conceptual Development series Core systems of number , 2004 .

[71]  Takashi Hanakawa,et al.  Differential activity in the premotor cortex subdivisions in humans during mental calculation and verbal rehearsal tasks: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study , 2003, Neuroscience Letters.

[72]  Takashi Hanakawa,et al.  Neural correlates underlying mental calculation in abacus experts: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study , 2003, NeuroImage.

[73]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  Atypical trajectories of number development: a neuroconstructivist perspective , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[74]  Avishai Henik,et al.  The development of internal representations of magnitude and their association with Arabic numerals. , 2002, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[75]  J. Tanaka,et al.  A Neural Basis for Expert Object Recognition , 2001, Psychological science.

[76]  L Girelli,et al.  The development of automaticity in accessing number magnitude. , 2000, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[77]  D. Berch,et al.  Extracting parity and magnitude from Arabic numerals: developmental changes in number processing and mental representation. , 1999, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[78]  E. Spelke,et al.  Sources of mathematical thinking: behavioral and brain-imaging evidence. , 1999, Science.

[79]  J. Pernier,et al.  ERP Manifestations of Processing Printed Words at Different Psycholinguistic Levels: Time Course and Scalp Distribution , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[80]  M. Posner,et al.  Brain mechanisms of quantity are similar in 5-year-old children and adults. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[81]  G. Comi,et al.  IFCN standards for digital recording of clinical EEG. International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology. , 1998, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[82]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  The Organization of Brain Activations in Number Comparison: Event-Related Potentials and the Additive-Factors Method , 1996, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[83]  G. Mangun,et al.  Luminance and spatial attention effects on early visual processing. , 1995, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[84]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Development of Elementary Numerical Abilities: A Neuronal Model , 1993, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[85]  J. Stigler “Mental abacus”: The effect of abacus training on Chinese children's mental calculation , 1984, Cognitive Psychology.

[86]  A. Henik,et al.  Is three greater than five: The relation between physical and semantic size in comparison tasks , 1982, Memory & cognition.

[87]  E. M. Duncan,et al.  Isolating the effects of symbolic distance, and semantic congruity in comparative judgments: An additive-factors analysis , 1980, Memory & cognition.

[88]  Giyoo Hatano,et al.  Performance of expert abacus operators , 1977, Cognition.

[89]  ROBERT S. MOYER,et al.  Time required for Judgements of Numerical Inequality , 1967, Nature.