Feature attention for binocular disparity in primate area MT depends on tuning strength.

Attending to a stimulus modulates the responses of sensory neurons that represent features of that stimulus, a phenomenon named "feature attention." For example, attending to a stimulus containing upward motion enhances the responses of upward-preferring direction-selective neurons in the middle temporal area (MT) and suppresses the responses of downward-preferring neurons, even when the attended stimulus is outside of the spatial receptive fields of the recorded neurons (Treue S, Martinez-Trujillo JC. Nature 399: 575-579, 1999). This modulation renders the representation of sensory information across a neuronal population more selective for the features present in the attended stimulus (Martinez-Trujillo JC, Treue S. Curr Biol 14: 744-751, 2004). We hypothesized that if feature attention modulates neurons according to their tuning preferences, it should also be sensitive to their tuning strength, which is the magnitude of the difference in responses to preferred and null stimuli. We measured how the effects of feature attention on MT neurons in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) depended on the relationship between features-in our case, direction of motion and binocular disparity-of the attended stimulus and a neuron's tuning for those features. We found that, as for direction, attention to stimuli containing binocular disparity cues modulated the responses of MT neurons and that the magnitude of the modulation depended on both a neuron's tuning preferences and its tuning strength. Our results suggest that modulation by feature attention may depend not just on which features a neuron represents but also on how well the neuron represents those features.

[1]  W. Newsome,et al.  Neuronal and psychophysical sensitivity to motion signals in extrastriate area MST of the macaque monkey , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[2]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  Dynamics of neuronal responses in macaque MT and VIP during motion detection , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[3]  R. Born,et al.  Timescales of Sensory- and Decision-Related Activity in the Middle Temporal and Medial Superior Temporal Areas , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[4]  Stefan Treue,et al.  Combining spatial and feature-based attention within the receptive field of MT neurons , 2009, Vision Research.

[5]  G. DeAngelis,et al.  Organization of Disparity-Selective Neurons in Macaque Area MT , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[6]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  Feature-based attention in visual cortex , 2006, Trends in Neurosciences.

[7]  D. Fitzpatrick,et al.  Orientation Selectivity and the Arrangement of Horizontal Connections in Tree Shrew Striate Cortex , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[8]  Robert Desimone,et al.  Parallel and Serial Neural Mechanisms for Visual Search in Macaque Area V4 , 2005, Science.

[9]  Stefan Treue,et al.  Feature-based attention influences motion processing gain in macaque visual cortex , 1999, Nature.

[10]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  Topographic organization of the middle temporal visual area in the macaque monkey: Representational biases and the relationship to callosal connections and myeloarchitectonic boundaries , 1987, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[11]  G. DeAngelis,et al.  Contribution of Area MT to Stereoscopic Depth Perception Choice-Related Response Modulations Reflect Task Strategy , 2004, Neuron.

[12]  Winrich A. Freiwald,et al.  Attention to Surfaces Modulates Motion Processing in Extrastriate Area MT , 2007, Neuron.

[13]  Robert Desimone,et al.  Feature-Based Attention in the Frontal Eye Field and Area V4 during Visual Search , 2011, Neuron.

[14]  L. Busse,et al.  Attention to the Color of a Moving Stimulus Modulates Motion-Signal Processing in Macaque Area MT: Evidence for a Unified Attentional System , 2009, Front. Syst. Neurosci..

[15]  Julio Martinez-Trujillo Searching for the Neural Mechanisms of Feature-Based Attention in the Primate Brain , 2011, Neuron.

[16]  J Allman,et al.  Direction- and Velocity-Specific Responses from beyond the Classical Receptive Field in the Middle Temporal Visual Area (MT) , 1985, Perception.

[17]  J. Movshon,et al.  A computational analysis of the relationship between neuronal and behavioral responses to visual motion , 1996, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[18]  D. Bradley,et al.  Neural population code for fine perceptual decisions in area MT , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[19]  L. Chelazzi,et al.  Neurons in Area V4 of the Macaque Translate Attended Visual Features into Behaviorally Relevant Categories , 2007, Neuron.

[20]  R. Desimone,et al.  Columnar organization of directionally selective cells in visual area MT of the macaque. , 1984, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  K. H. Britten,et al.  A relationship between behavioral choice and the visual responses of neurons in macaque MT , 1996, Visual Neuroscience.

[22]  J. Maunsell,et al.  Using Neuronal Populations to Study the Mechanisms Underlying Spatial and Feature Attention , 2011, Neuron.

[23]  R. Desimone,et al.  Local precision of visuotopic organization in the middle temporal area (MT) of the macaque , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[24]  K Nakayama,et al.  Visual attention to surfaces in three-dimensional space. , 1995, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[25]  W. Newsome,et al.  Context-Dependent Changes in Functional Circuitry in Visual Area MT , 2008, Neuron.

[26]  Gregory C DeAngelis,et al.  Coding of horizontal disparity and velocity by MT neurons in the alert macaque. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  S. Treue,et al.  Feature-Based Attention Increases the Selectivity of Population Responses in Primate Visual Cortex , 2004, Current Biology.

[28]  R. Duncan Luce,et al.  Response Times: Their Role in Inferring Elementary Mental Organization , 1986 .

[29]  Richard T Born,et al.  Joint tuning for direction of motion and binocular disparity in macaque MT is largely separable. , 2013, Journal of neurophysiology.

[30]  D C Van Essen,et al.  Functional properties of neurons in middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey. I. Selectivity for stimulus direction, speed, and orientation. , 1983, Journal of neurophysiology.

[31]  Felix Wichmann,et al.  The psychometric function: II. Bootstrap-based confidence intervals and sampling , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.

[32]  Stefan Treue,et al.  Visual attention: of features and transparent surfaces , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[33]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  Functional properties of neurons in middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey. II. Binocular interactions and sensitivity to binocular disparity. , 1983, Journal of neurophysiology.

[34]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Multiple visual areas in the caudal superior temporal sulcus of the macaque , 1986, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[35]  C. Law,et al.  Reinforcement learning can account for associative and perceptual learning on a visual decision task , 2009, Nature Neuroscience.

[36]  C. Law,et al.  Neural correlates of perceptual learning in a sensory-motor, but not a sensory, cortical area , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[37]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  Attention to both space and feature modulates neuronal responses in macaque area V4. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[38]  Bruce Cumming,et al.  Correlations between the activity of sensory neurons and behavior: how much do they tell us about a neuron's causality? , 2010, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[39]  Paul S Khayat,et al.  Attention Differentially Modulates Similar Neuronal Responses Evoked by Varying Contrast and Direction Stimuli in Area MT , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[40]  B. Motter Neural correlates of attentive selection for color or luminance in extrastriate area V4 , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.