VOT Production in English and Arabic Bilingual and Monolingual Children

This paper presents findings from an instrumental study of Voice Onset Time (VOT) production in 3 English-Arabic bilingual children and 3 monolingual controls from each language. The subjects were all aged between 5 and 10, and the recordings were made in England and Lebanon. The aim was to find out whether bilinguals acquire separate VOT patterns for each language and whether these patterns are parallel to the monolingual ones. Results showed that the 3 bilinguals did acquire distinct VOT patterns for each language, but the patterns did not always resemble monolingual ones. The main affected feature was voicing lead in Arabic, as it was often replaced with short lag. However, similar behaviour was found in the young monolingual children, which helped interpret some of the bilingual patterns in terms of normal developmental features rather than resorting to the usual explanations based on ‘language interference’. Results from both monolinguals and bilinguals showed that the differences between the two languages are finer than the three VOT categories suggest, particularly with respect to short lag. Finally developmental changes for two of the bilingual subjects over a period of 18 months revealed the importance of an interrelation between input and age for the acquisition of two independent phonological systems.

[1]  G. Docherty The Timing of Voicing in British English Obstruents , 1992 .

[2]  Bruce L. Smith,et al.  A Longitudinal Study of the Development of Temporal Properties of Speech Production: Data from 4 Children , 1999, Phonetica.

[3]  Z. Bond,et al.  VOT del Espanol to English: Comparison of a Language-Disordered and Normal Child. , 1980 .

[4]  Taehong Cho,et al.  Variation and universals in VOT: evidence from 18 languages , 1999 .

[5]  J. Flege PHONETIC APPROXIMATION IN SECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION1 , 1980 .

[6]  D. Klatt Voice onset time, frication, and aspiration in word-initial consonant clusters. , 1975, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[7]  I. Watson Language processing in bilingual children: Phonological processing in two languages , 1991 .

[8]  R. Port,et al.  Cross-Language Phonetic Interference: Arabic to English , 1981 .

[9]  J. Fokes,et al.  Voice onset time: The development of Spanish/English distinction in normal and language disordered children , 1981 .

[10]  Marlys A. Macken,et al.  The acquisition of the voicing contrast in Spanish: a phonetic and phonological study of word-initial stop consonants , 1980, Journal of Child Language.

[11]  J. Flege,et al.  Production and perception of English stops by native Spanish speakers , 1987 .

[12]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  A study of voicing in Lebanese Arabic , 1977 .

[13]  L. Lisker,et al.  Some Effects of Context On Voice Onset Time in English Stops , 1967, Language and speech.

[14]  Arthur S. Abramson,et al.  DISTINCTIVE FEATURES AND LARYNGEAL CONTROL , 1971 .

[15]  J. Elman,et al.  Perceptual switching in bilinguals , 1977 .

[16]  R. Koenigsknecht,et al.  Development of the voicing contrast: a comparison of voice onset time in stop preception and production. , 1976, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[17]  Emily Krasinski,et al.  The development of segmental phonological production in a bilingual child , 1994, Journal of Child Language.

[18]  Gillian Brown,et al.  Listening to spoken English , 1977 .

[19]  D B Pisoni,et al.  Discrimination of voice onset time by human infants: new findings and implications for the effects of early experience. , 1981, Child development.

[20]  Marcello Marcelino,et al.  SOME CHARACTERISTICS OF VOT IN PLOSIVES PRODUCED BY SPEAKERS OF ENGLISH AND PORTUGUESE , 1999 .

[21]  Raymond D. Kent,et al.  Acoustic Analysis of Speech , 2009 .

[22]  L. Lisker,et al.  A Cross-Language Study of Voicing in Initial Stops: Acoustical Measurements , 1964 .

[23]  Diane Kewley-Port,et al.  Early apical stop production: A voice onset time analysis , 1974 .

[24]  Adrian Fourcin,et al.  Cross‐language study of speech‐pattern learning , 1977 .

[25]  E. Bialystok Language Processing in Bilingual Children , 1991 .

[26]  Anne Cutler,et al.  Limits on bilingualism , 1989, Nature.

[27]  P. Kerswill Children, adolescents, and language change , 1996, Language Variation and Change.

[28]  Paul Foulkes,et al.  TRACKING THE EMERGENCE OF SOCIOPHONETIC VARIATION , 1999 .

[29]  G. Weismer,et al.  Closure duration of stop consonants , 1983 .

[30]  Margaret Deuchar,et al.  Early bilingual acquisition of the voicing contrast in English and Spanish , 1996 .

[31]  CATEGORIZATION OF BILABIAL STOP CONSONANTS BY BILINGUAL SPEAKERS OF ENGLISH AND POLISH , 1999 .

[32]  P. Ladefoged,et al.  The sounds of the world's languages , 1996 .

[33]  G D Allen,et al.  How the young French child avoids the pre-voicing problem for word-initial voiced stops , 1985, Journal of Child Language.

[34]  E. Zurif,et al.  The acquisition of a new phonological contrast: the case of stop consonants in French-English bilinguals. , 1973, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[35]  L. Lisker,et al.  Letter: Is it VOT or a first-formant transition detector? , 1975, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[36]  Robert F. Port,et al.  Segmental features and implementation in acquisition of English by Arabic speakers , 1983 .

[37]  Marlys A. Macken,et al.  The acquisition of the voicing contrast in English: a study of voice onset time in word-initial stop consonants , 1980, Journal of Child Language.

[38]  V. Hazan,et al.  Perception and Production of a Voicing Contrast by French-English Bilinguals , 1993 .

[39]  Michael Kenstowicz,et al.  Phonology In Generative Grammar , 1994 .

[40]  Gary Weismer,et al.  Control of the voicing distinction for intervocalic stops and fricatives: some data and theoretical considerations , 1980 .

[41]  K. Stevens,et al.  Role of formant transitions in the voiced-voiceless distinction for stops. , 1974, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.