N1-P2: Neural markers of temporal expectation and response discrimination in interval timing.

Humans use temporal regularities in their daily life to act in accordance with future events in the most efficient way. To achieve this, humans build temporal expectations and determine a template action that is in line with those expectations. In this temporal trisection study, we aimed to study the neurophysiological counterparts of temporal expectation and response discrimination. We investigated amplitude variations of early event-related potentials (ERPs) while manipulating time intervals. We measured temporal expectation-related attenuation of neural activity and response discrimination processes in N1 and P2 ERP components. Results showed that the amplitude of the N1 component was attenuated for the predicted task-relevant temporal location of a response decision. The P2 amplitude, in contrast, was enhanced for a discriminated response in comparison to a template response. The present study supports a link between the different functional associations of the N1 and P2 components within the requirements of a timing task. N1-related amplitude modulation can determine a change in expectation level during timing. The amplitude regulation of the P2 component, in contrast, explains temporal discrimination in both expected and unexpected temporal locations. In addition to expectation‑related modulation, our results suggest an additional regulation of the N1 amplitude that is linked to attention. The effect was observed in instances that included a prediction error of a task-relevant temporal location for a response decision. In conclusion, our study contributes to the growing neurocognitive literature on interval timing by capturing different aspects of a timing task; namely, N1-related expectation and P2-related response discrimination processes.

[1]  A. Nobre,et al.  Where and When to Pay Attention: The Neural Systems for Directing Attention to Spatial Locations and to Time Intervals as Revealed by Both PET and fMRI , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[2]  Virginie van Wassenhove,et al.  Duration estimation entails predicting when , 2015, NeuroImage.

[3]  N. Prins Psychophysics: A Practical Introduction , 2009 .

[4]  J. Coull Neural Substrates of Mounting Temporal Expectation , 2009, PLoS biology.

[5]  R. Knight,et al.  Anatomical substrates of auditory selective attention: behavioral and electrophysiological effects of posterior association cortex lesions. , 1993, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[6]  Helen C. Barron,et al.  Repetition suppression: a means to index neural representations using BOLD? , 2016, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[7]  Zhong-Lin Lu,et al.  Visual Psychophysics: From Laboratory to Theory , 2013 .

[8]  David M. Eagleman,et al.  Is Subjective Duration a Signature of Coding Efficiency , 2010 .

[9]  Hedderik van Rijn,et al.  Decoupling Interval Timing and Climbing Neural Activity: A Dissociation between CNV and N1P2 Amplitudes , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[10]  G. Woodman,et al.  Event-related potential studies of attention , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[11]  Tadeusz W. Kononowicz,et al.  Slow Potentials in Time Estimation: The Role of Temporal Accumulation and Habituation , 2011, Front. Integr. Neurosci..

[12]  Anna Grabowska,et al.  Event-related potentials in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: an investigation using an auditory oddball task. , 2012, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[13]  C. Jacques,et al.  The N170 : understanding the time-course of face perception in the human brain , 2011 .

[14]  Q. Fu,et al.  The adaptive pattern of the auditory N1 peak revealed by standardized low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography , 2011, Brain Research.

[15]  R. Desimone,et al.  Parallel neuronal mechanisms for short-term memory. , 1994, Science.

[16]  Warren H. Meck,et al.  Timing and Time Perception : A Critical Review of Neural Timing Signatures Before, During, and After the To‐Be‐Timed Interval , 2018 .

[17]  R. Hari,et al.  Auditory attention affects two different areas in the human supratemporal cortex. , 1991, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[18]  E. Wascher,et al.  The role of inhibition for working memory processes: ERP evidence from a short-term storage task. , 2018, Psychophysiology.

[19]  W. Matthews Can we use verbal estimation to dissect the internal clock? Differentiating the effects of pacemaker rate, switch latencies, and judgment processes , 2011, Behavioural Processes.

[20]  J. Gold,et al.  How mechanisms of perceptual decision-making affect the psychometric function , 2013, Progress in Neurobiology.

[21]  D. Tucker,et al.  Frontal evaluation and posterior representation in target detection. , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[22]  R. Oades,et al.  Auditory event-related potentials (ERPs) and mismatch negativity (MMN) in healthy children and those with attention-deficit or tourette/tic symptoms , 1996, Biological Psychology.

[23]  R. Barker,et al.  Time on timing: Dissociating premature responding from interval sensitivity in Parkinson's disease , 2016, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[24]  B. Milliken,et al.  Endogenous temporal orienting of attention in detection and discrimination tasks , 2004, Perception & psychophysics.

[25]  Simon Finnigan,et al.  ERP measures indicate both attention and working memory encoding decrements in aging. , 2011, Psychophysiology.

[26]  J. Connolly,et al.  Assessment of working memory abilities using an event-related brain potential (ERP)-compatible digit span backward task , 2005, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[27]  S A Hillyard,et al.  Temporal dynamics of human auditory selective attention. , 1988, Psychophysiology.

[28]  Jeesun Kim,et al.  The Processing of Attended and Predicted Sounds in Time , 2016, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[29]  Michela Sarlo,et al.  Automatic Temporal Expectancy: A High-Density Event-Related Potential Study , 2013, PloS one.

[30]  Franck Vidal,et al.  The supplementary motor area in motor and sensory timing: evidence from slow brain potential changes , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[31]  Vani Pariyadath,et al.  Brief subjective durations contract with repetition. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[32]  T. Picton,et al.  The N1 wave of the human electric and magnetic response to sound: a review and an analysis of the component structure. , 1987, Psychophysiology.

[33]  F. Mauguière,et al.  Revisiting the oddball paradigm. Non-target vs neutral stimuli and the evaluation of ERP attentional effects , 1992, Neuropsychologia.

[34]  M. Posner,et al.  Frontal and inferior temporal cortical activity in visual target detection: Evidence from high spatially sampled event-related potentials , 1996, Brain Topography.

[35]  R. McCarley,et al.  Auditory ERPs to non-target stimuli in schizophrenia: relationship to probability, task-demands, and target ERPs. , 1994, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[36]  L. Allan,et al.  Temporal bisection with trial referents , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.

[37]  Mari Tervaniemi,et al.  Music Training Enhances Rapid Neural Plasticity of N1 and P2 Source Activation for Unattended Sounds , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[38]  E Donchin,et al.  A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. , 1983, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[39]  Kwun Kei Ng,et al.  Temporal Accumulation and Decision Processes in the Duration Bisection Task Revealed by Contingent Negative Variation , 2011, Front. Integr. Neurosci..

[40]  Luc H. Arnal,et al.  Cortical oscillations and sensory predictions , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[41]  R M Church,et al.  Properties of the Internal Clock a , 1984, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[42]  K. Crowley,et al.  A review of the evidence for P2 being an independent component process: age, sleep and modality , 2004, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[43]  Adrian M. Owen,et al.  Dissociable endogenous and exogenous attention in disorders of consciousness☆ , 2013, NeuroImage: Clinical.

[44]  Kathy Dujardin,et al.  Effects of stimulus-driven and goal-directed attention on prepulse inhibition of the cortical responses to an auditory pulse , 2014, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[45]  E. Grigorenko,et al.  Attentional But Not Pre-Attentive Neural Measures of Auditory Discrimination Are Atypical in Children With Developmental Language Disorder , 2014, Developmental neuropsychology.

[46]  Anna C Nobre,et al.  Neural modulation by regularity and passage of time. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[47]  Lorraine G. Allan,et al.  Are the Referents Remembered in Temporal Bisection , 2002 .

[48]  Franck Vidal,et al.  Time processing reflected by EEG surface Laplacians , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[49]  A. Nobre,et al.  The hazards of time , 2007, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[50]  W. Matthews,et al.  Repetition, expectation, and the perception of time , 2016, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences.

[51]  R. Dowman The Pain-Evoked P2 Is Not a P3a Event-Related Potential , 2004, Brain Topography.

[52]  A. Nobre,et al.  Multiple mechanisms of selective attention: differential modulation of stimulus processing by attention to space or time , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[53]  N. Boutros,et al.  P 50 , N 100 , and P 200 sensory gating : Relationships with behavioral inhibition , attention , and working memory , 2010 .