N1-P2: Neural markers of temporal expectation and response discrimination in interval timing.
暂无分享,去创建一个
Annette Hohenberger | Halil Duzcu | Tolga Esat Özkurt | Igor Mapelli | T. Özkurt | A. Hohenberger | Igor Mapelli | Halil Duzcu
[1] A. Nobre,et al. Where and When to Pay Attention: The Neural Systems for Directing Attention to Spatial Locations and to Time Intervals as Revealed by Both PET and fMRI , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[2] Virginie van Wassenhove,et al. Duration estimation entails predicting when , 2015, NeuroImage.
[3] N. Prins. Psychophysics: A Practical Introduction , 2009 .
[4] J. Coull. Neural Substrates of Mounting Temporal Expectation , 2009, PLoS biology.
[5] R. Knight,et al. Anatomical substrates of auditory selective attention: behavioral and electrophysiological effects of posterior association cortex lesions. , 1993, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[6] Helen C. Barron,et al. Repetition suppression: a means to index neural representations using BOLD? , 2016, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[7] Zhong-Lin Lu,et al. Visual Psychophysics: From Laboratory to Theory , 2013 .
[8] David M. Eagleman,et al. Is Subjective Duration a Signature of Coding Efficiency , 2010 .
[9] Hedderik van Rijn,et al. Decoupling Interval Timing and Climbing Neural Activity: A Dissociation between CNV and N1P2 Amplitudes , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[10] G. Woodman,et al. Event-related potential studies of attention , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[11] Tadeusz W. Kononowicz,et al. Slow Potentials in Time Estimation: The Role of Temporal Accumulation and Habituation , 2011, Front. Integr. Neurosci..
[12] Anna Grabowska,et al. Event-related potentials in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: an investigation using an auditory oddball task. , 2012, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[13] C. Jacques,et al. The N170 : understanding the time-course of face perception in the human brain , 2011 .
[14] Q. Fu,et al. The adaptive pattern of the auditory N1 peak revealed by standardized low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography , 2011, Brain Research.
[15] R. Desimone,et al. Parallel neuronal mechanisms for short-term memory. , 1994, Science.
[16] Warren H. Meck,et al. Timing and Time Perception : A Critical Review of Neural Timing Signatures Before, During, and After the To‐Be‐Timed Interval , 2018 .
[17] R. Hari,et al. Auditory attention affects two different areas in the human supratemporal cortex. , 1991, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[18] E. Wascher,et al. The role of inhibition for working memory processes: ERP evidence from a short-term storage task. , 2018, Psychophysiology.
[19] W. Matthews. Can we use verbal estimation to dissect the internal clock? Differentiating the effects of pacemaker rate, switch latencies, and judgment processes , 2011, Behavioural Processes.
[20] J. Gold,et al. How mechanisms of perceptual decision-making affect the psychometric function , 2013, Progress in Neurobiology.
[21] D. Tucker,et al. Frontal evaluation and posterior representation in target detection. , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[22] R. Oades,et al. Auditory event-related potentials (ERPs) and mismatch negativity (MMN) in healthy children and those with attention-deficit or tourette/tic symptoms , 1996, Biological Psychology.
[23] R. Barker,et al. Time on timing: Dissociating premature responding from interval sensitivity in Parkinson's disease , 2016, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.
[24] B. Milliken,et al. Endogenous temporal orienting of attention in detection and discrimination tasks , 2004, Perception & psychophysics.
[25] Simon Finnigan,et al. ERP measures indicate both attention and working memory encoding decrements in aging. , 2011, Psychophysiology.
[26] J. Connolly,et al. Assessment of working memory abilities using an event-related brain potential (ERP)-compatible digit span backward task , 2005, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[27] S A Hillyard,et al. Temporal dynamics of human auditory selective attention. , 1988, Psychophysiology.
[28] Jeesun Kim,et al. The Processing of Attended and Predicted Sounds in Time , 2016, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[29] Michela Sarlo,et al. Automatic Temporal Expectancy: A High-Density Event-Related Potential Study , 2013, PloS one.
[30] Franck Vidal,et al. The supplementary motor area in motor and sensory timing: evidence from slow brain potential changes , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.
[31] Vani Pariyadath,et al. Brief subjective durations contract with repetition. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[32] T. Picton,et al. The N1 wave of the human electric and magnetic response to sound: a review and an analysis of the component structure. , 1987, Psychophysiology.
[33] F. Mauguière,et al. Revisiting the oddball paradigm. Non-target vs neutral stimuli and the evaluation of ERP attentional effects , 1992, Neuropsychologia.
[34] M. Posner,et al. Frontal and inferior temporal cortical activity in visual target detection: Evidence from high spatially sampled event-related potentials , 1996, Brain Topography.
[35] R. McCarley,et al. Auditory ERPs to non-target stimuli in schizophrenia: relationship to probability, task-demands, and target ERPs. , 1994, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[36] L. Allan,et al. Temporal bisection with trial referents , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.
[37] Mari Tervaniemi,et al. Music Training Enhances Rapid Neural Plasticity of N1 and P2 Source Activation for Unattended Sounds , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[38] E Donchin,et al. A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. , 1983, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[39] Kwun Kei Ng,et al. Temporal Accumulation and Decision Processes in the Duration Bisection Task Revealed by Contingent Negative Variation , 2011, Front. Integr. Neurosci..
[40] Luc H. Arnal,et al. Cortical oscillations and sensory predictions , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[41] R M Church,et al. Properties of the Internal Clock a , 1984, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[42] K. Crowley,et al. A review of the evidence for P2 being an independent component process: age, sleep and modality , 2004, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[43] Adrian M. Owen,et al. Dissociable endogenous and exogenous attention in disorders of consciousness☆ , 2013, NeuroImage: Clinical.
[44] Kathy Dujardin,et al. Effects of stimulus-driven and goal-directed attention on prepulse inhibition of the cortical responses to an auditory pulse , 2014, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[45] E. Grigorenko,et al. Attentional But Not Pre-Attentive Neural Measures of Auditory Discrimination Are Atypical in Children With Developmental Language Disorder , 2014, Developmental neuropsychology.
[46] Anna C Nobre,et al. Neural modulation by regularity and passage of time. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.
[47] Lorraine G. Allan,et al. Are the Referents Remembered in Temporal Bisection , 2002 .
[48] Franck Vidal,et al. Time processing reflected by EEG surface Laplacians , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.
[49] A. Nobre,et al. The hazards of time , 2007, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[50] W. Matthews,et al. Repetition, expectation, and the perception of time , 2016, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences.
[51] R. Dowman. The Pain-Evoked P2 Is Not a P3a Event-Related Potential , 2004, Brain Topography.
[52] A. Nobre,et al. Multiple mechanisms of selective attention: differential modulation of stimulus processing by attention to space or time , 2002, Neuropsychologia.
[53] N. Boutros,et al. P 50 , N 100 , and P 200 sensory gating : Relationships with behavioral inhibition , attention , and working memory , 2010 .