Chosen striking location and the user-tool-environment system.

Controlling a hand-held tool requires that the tool user bring the tool into contact with an environmental surface in a task-appropriate manner. This, in turn, requires applying muscular forces so as to overcome how the object resists being moved about its various axes. Perceived properties of hand-held objects tend to be constrained by inertial variables that describe such resistance to movement. In 3 experiments, the authors investigated whether participants choose different striking locations on striking implements when using that implement under different constraints. The results suggest that the constraints of the striking task and the grasp position on the striking implement together contribute to the chosen striking location. Furthermore, such choices are constrained by inertial variables that reflect combinations of these constraints.

[1]  Jeffrey B Wagman,et al.  Haptically creating affordances: the user-tool interface. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[2]  J. Flach The Ecology of Human-Machine Systems I: Introduction , 1990 .

[3]  Kim J. Vicente,et al.  The Human Factor - Revolutionizing the way people live with technology , 2004 .

[4]  A. Smitsman,et al.  Affordances, perceptual complexity, and the development of tool use. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[5]  M W Riley,et al.  The Effects of Handle Shape and Size on Exerted Forces , 1986, Human factors.

[6]  Kevin Shockley,et al.  Metamers in the haptic perception of heaviness and moveableness , 2004, Perception & psychophysics.

[7]  R DRILLIS,et al.  The Theory of Striking Tools , 1963, Human factors.

[8]  C. Pagano Dynamic Touch in Varying Media and for Proprioception , 2004 .

[9]  Jeffrey B. Wagman,et al.  Affordances and Inertial Constraints on Tool Use , 2001 .

[10]  T J Armstrong,et al.  Repetitive Trauma Disorders: Job Evaluation and Design , 1986, Human factors.

[11]  M. Turvey,et al.  Ecological foundations of cognition. I: Symmetry and specificity of animal-environment systems , 1999 .

[12]  John M. Flach An Ecological Alternative to Eggsucking , 1989 .

[13]  D. Katz The World of Touch , 1989 .

[14]  M. Turvey,et al.  Affordance, proper function, and the physical basis of perceived heaviness , 1999, Cognition.

[15]  G. Burton,et al.  Nonvisual judgment of the crossability of path gaps. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[16]  M. Turvey,et al.  Somatosensory attunement to the rigid body laws , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[17]  John M. Flach,et al.  Control Theory for Humans: Quantitative Approaches To Modeling Performance , 2002 .

[18]  I. Kingma,et al.  The Inertia Tensor Versus Static Moment and Mass in Perceiving Length and Heaviness of Hand-Wielded Rods , 2002 .

[19]  Michael T. Turvey,et al.  Physics and Psychology of the Muscle Sense , 2004 .

[20]  Sarah E. Berger,et al.  Infants use handrails as tools in a locomotor task. , 2003, Developmental psychology.

[21]  Peter J Beek,et al.  Which mechanical invariants are associated with the perception of length and heaviness of a nonvisible handheld rod? Testing the inertia tensor hypothesis. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[22]  K H Kroemer,et al.  Coupling the Hand with the Handle: An Improved Notation of Touch, Grip, and Grasp , 1986, Human factors.

[23]  Thomas H. Rockwell,et al.  An experimental evaluation of method and tool effects in spike maul use , 1986 .

[24]  Michael T. Turvey,et al.  Aging and the perception of a racket's sweet spot , 2000 .

[25]  John M. Kennedy,et al.  Space, haptics and the blind , 1992 .

[26]  A. Noē Direct Perception , 2022 .

[27]  A. Smitsman,et al.  Geometries and Dynamics of a Rod Determine How It Is Used for Reaching , 2003, Journal of motor behavior.

[28]  David B. Miller,et al.  Nested reciprocities: the organism-environment system in perception-action and development. , 2003, Developmental psychobiology.

[29]  Lynette A. Hart,et al.  Cognitive behaviour in Asian elephants: use and modification of branches for fly switching , 2001, Animal Behaviour.

[30]  K. J. Vicente,et al.  The Ecology of Human-Machine Systems II: Mediating 'Direct Perception' in Complex Work Domains , 1990 .

[31]  E. Gibson,et al.  Principles of Perceptual Learning and Development , 1973 .

[32]  Daniel A. Cristol,et al.  Avian prey-dropping behavior. II. American crows and walnuts , 1999 .

[33]  Raoul M Bongers,et al.  Variations of Tool and Task Characteristics Reveal That Tool-Use Postures Are Anticipated , 2004, Journal of motor behavior.

[34]  Alex Kirlik,et al.  The Ecological Expert: acting to create information to guide action , 1998, Proceedings Fourth Annual Symposium on Human Interaction with Complex Systems.

[35]  Richard G. Carson,et al.  Spoon handling in two- to four year old children , 1997 .

[36]  S J Lederman,et al.  Manipulation with no or partial vision. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[37]  K. J. Vicente,et al.  Cognitive Work Analysis: Toward Safe, Productive, and Healthy Computer-Based Work , 1999 .

[38]  Christopher C. Pagano,et al.  Eigenvectors of the Inertia Tensor and Perceiving the Orientations of Limbs and Objects , 1998 .

[39]  Jack A. Kraft A 1961 Compilation and Brief History of Human Factors Research in Business and Industry , 1961 .

[40]  L. Kaufman,et al.  Handbook of perception and human performance , 1986 .

[41]  E. Reed The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1989 .

[42]  Anil Mital,et al.  Comparison of Maximum Volitional Torque Exertion Capabilities of Males and Females Using Common Hand Tools , 1986 .

[43]  M. Turvey Affordances and Prospective Control: An Outline of the Ontology , 1992 .

[44]  Morton A. Heller,et al.  Touch, representation, and blindness , 2000 .

[45]  M T Turvey,et al.  Tensorial basis to the constancy of perceived object extent over variations of dynamic touch , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[46]  Jeffrey B. Wagman,et al.  Implications of Dynamic Touch for Human Factors/Ergonomics: Contributions from Ecological Psychology , 2004 .

[47]  J. Valsiner,et al.  Handbook of developmental psychology. , 2003 .

[48]  Jeffrey B. Wagman Human Factors Implications of Controlling User-Tool-Environment Interfaces , 2004 .

[49]  Ad W. Smitsman,et al.  Tool use and tool making: a dynamical developmental perspective , 2003 .

[50]  Gregory Burton,et al.  Contact and Posture in Nonvisual Judgment of Gap Crossability , 1997 .

[51]  J G Kreifeldt,et al.  Moment of inertia: psychophysical study of an overlooked sensation. , 1979, Science.

[52]  M T Turvey,et al.  Perceiving the Sweet Spot , 1999, Perception.

[53]  H. Brody,et al.  Tennis science for tennis players , 1987 .

[54]  A. Smitsman,et al.  The development of tool use: Changing boundaries between organism and environment. , 1997 .

[55]  E. Reed Encountering the world: Toward an ecological psychology. , 1997 .

[56]  J. Gibson The Senses Considered As Perceptual Systems , 1967 .

[57]  M. Turvey Dynamic touch. , 1996, The American psychologist.

[58]  R J DRILLIS,et al.  Folk Norms and Biomechanics , 1963, Human factors.

[59]  M T Turvey,et al.  Attunement, Calibration, and Exploration in Fast Haptic Perceptual Learning , 2001, Journal of motor behavior.