Effects of motion on blur discrimination

Blur-discrimination thresholds for moving, Gaussian-blurred edges were measured. Our results show that motion produces equivalent spatial blur. The velocity dependence of this blur is linear, and its extent can be predicted by a temporal impulse response with a standard deviation of ∼5 ms in normal room light. This blur is most likely to result from cameralike summation and not from the use of larger spatial filters for moving than for stationary objects. As a function of reference blur, blur-discrimination thresholds approach asymptotically a Weber’s-law relation. Motion-deblurring models are discussed in view of our findings, and an alternative model for temporal integration is proposed.

[1]  D C Van Essen,et al.  Shifter circuits: a computational strategy for dynamic aspects of visual processing. , 1987, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[2]  A. Hodgkin,et al.  Reconstruction of the electrical responses of turtle cones to flashes and steps of light , 1974, The Journal of physiology.

[3]  S. Watamaniuk,et al.  Visible Persistence is Reduced by Fixed-Trajectory Motion but Not by Random Motion , 1992, Perception.

[4]  Spatial-frequency discrimination of drifting gratings , 1990, Vision Research.

[5]  H. Wilson,et al.  Modified line-element theory for spatial-frequency and width discrimination. , 1984, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[6]  S P McKee,et al.  Discrimination of time: comparison of foveal and peripheral sensitivity. , 1984, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[7]  K R Nielsen,et al.  Application of a computable model of human spatial vision to phase discrimination. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[8]  J Z Levinson,et al.  Flicker Fusion Phenomena , 1968, Science.

[9]  M. Morgan,et al.  Motion-deblurring in human vision , 1989, Nature.

[10]  H. Barlow Temporal and spatial summation in human vision at different background intensities , 1958, The Journal of physiology.

[11]  H. Barlow,et al.  Three factors limiting the reliable detection of light by retinal ganglion cells of the cat , 1969, The Journal of physiology.

[12]  R. Hess,et al.  The functional area for summation to threshold for sinusoidal gratings , 1978, Vision Research.

[13]  F. Campbell,et al.  Spatial-frequency discrimination in human vision. , 1970, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[14]  D. P. Andrews,et al.  APE: Adaptive probit estimation of psychometric functions , 1981 .

[15]  S. McKee,et al.  Steroscopic acuity for moving retinal images. , 1978, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[16]  R. J. Watt,et al.  Towards a general theory of the visual acuities for shape and spatial arrangement , 1984, Vision Research.

[17]  J. Robson,et al.  Probability summation and regional variation in contrast sensitivity across the visual field , 1981, Vision Research.

[18]  F. J. J. Blommaert,et al.  Temporal impulse and step responses of the human eye obtained psychophysically by means of a drift-correcting perturbation technique , 1981, Vision Research.

[19]  S Anderson,et al.  Visual allusions: Pictures of perception Nicholas Wade Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hove, East Sussex, UK, 1990, £24.95 , 1992 .

[20]  H. Barlow,et al.  Changes in the maintained discharge with adaptation level in the cat retina , 1969, The Journal of physiology.

[21]  Dodwell Pc,et al.  On perceptual clarity. , 1971 .

[22]  D. H. Kelly Theory of flicker and transient responses. I. Uniform fields. , 1971, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[23]  D. Burr Temporal summation of moving images by the human visual system , 1981, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[24]  DH Hubel,et al.  Psychophysical evidence for separate channels for the perception of form, color, movement, and depth , 1987, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[25]  S. McKee,et al.  Visual acuity in the presence of retinal-image motion. , 1975, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[26]  J. Bergen,et al.  Prediction of flicker sensitivities from temporal three-pulse data , 1985, Vision Research.

[27]  Suzanne P. McKee,et al.  Integration regions for visual hyperacuity , 1977, Vision Research.

[28]  P. Lennie,et al.  Spatial and temporal contrast sensitivities of neurones in lateral geniculate nucleus of macaque. , 1984, The Journal of physiology.

[29]  D. Burr,et al.  Seeing objects in motion , 1986, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[30]  D. H. Kelly Theory of flicker and transient responses. II. Counterphase gratings. , 1971, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[31]  A. Watson Derivation of the impulse response: comments on the method of Roufs and Blommaert , 1982, Vision Research.

[32]  Dennis M. Levi,et al.  Equivalent intrinsic blur in spatial vision , 1990, Vision Research.

[33]  On perceptual clarity. , 1971, Psychological review.

[34]  R. Watt,et al.  Spatial filters and the localization of luminance changes in human vision , 1984, Vision Research.

[35]  R. Watt,et al.  The recognition and representation of edge blur: Evidence for spatial primitives in human vision , 1983, Vision Research.

[36]  D. Marr,et al.  Smallest channel in early human vision. , 1980, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[37]  S. Klein,et al.  Sampling in spatial vision , 1986, Nature.

[38]  David R. Badcock,et al.  Resistance to positional noise in human vision , 1990, Nature.

[39]  David R. Badcock,et al.  The sensitivity of separation discrimination to spatiotemporal jitter , 1990, Vision Research.

[40]  R. Watt,et al.  A theory of the primitive spatial code in human vision , 1985, Vision Research.

[41]  G. Orban,et al.  Human velocity and direction discrimination measured with random dot patterns , 1988, Vision Research.