The development of English vowel perception in monolingual and bilingual infants: Neurophysiological correlates
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] J. Werker,et al. Infants listen for more phonetic detail in speech perception than in word-learning tasks , 1997, Nature.
[2] Valerie L. Shafer,et al. Maturation of Mismatch Negativity in Typically Developing Infants and Preschool Children , 2002, Ear and hearing.
[3] J. Werker,et al. Phonemic and phonetic factors in adult cross-language speech perception. , 1984, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[4] C. Pallier,et al. A limit on behavioral plasticity in speech perception , 1997, Cognition.
[5] James Emil Flege,et al. Pronunciation Proficiency in the First and Second Languages of Korean-English Bilinguals. , 2000 .
[6] K. Eklund,et al. Brain Responses to Changes in Speech Sound Durations Differ Between Infants With and Without Familial Risk for Dyslexia , 2002, Developmental neuropsychology.
[7] Laurel J. Trainor,et al. Maturation of cortical mismatch responses to occasional pitch change in early infancy: Effects of presentation rate and magnitude of change , 2009, Neuropsychologia.
[8] Paavo Alku,et al. Auditory discrimination profiles of speech sound changes in 6-year-old children as determined with the multi-feature MMN paradigm , 2009, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[9] Paavo Alku,et al. Mismatch negativity shows that 3–6-year-old children can learn to discriminate non-native speech sounds within two months , 2002, Neuroscience Letters.
[10] H G Vaughan,et al. Electrophysiological evidence of developmental changes in the duration of auditory sensory memory. , 1999, Developmental psychology.
[11] H Lyytinen,et al. Cortical responses of infants with and without a genetic risk for dyslexia: II. Group effects. , 1999, Neuroreport.
[12] R. Ilmoniemi,et al. Language-specific phoneme representations revealed by electric and magnetic brain responses , 1997, Nature.
[13] Paul Iverson,et al. Neural signatures of phonetic learning in adulthood: A magnetoencephalography study , 2009, NeuroImage.
[14] R. Näätänen,et al. Development of language-specific phoneme representations in the infant brain , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.
[15] J. Campos,et al. Auditory evoked potentials and sex-related differences in brain development , 1981, Brain and Language.
[16] Pavel Trofimovich,et al. The effect of perceived phonetic similarity on non-native sound learning by children and adults , 2006 .
[17] Mitchell Steinschneider,et al. Spectrotemporal analysis of evoked and induced electroencephalographic responses in primary auditory cortex (A1) of the awake monkey. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.
[18] I. R. MacKay,et al. PERCEIVING VOWELS IN A SECOND LANGUAGE , 2004, Studies in Second Language Acquisition.
[19] M. Dorman,et al. Neurophysiologic correlates of cross-language phonetic perception. , 2000, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[20] P. Huttenlocher,et al. Regional differences in synaptogenesis in human cerebral cortex , 1997, The Journal of comparative neurology.
[21] Linda Polka,et al. Language-experience facilitates discrimination of /d- / in monolingual and bilingual acquisition of English , 2006, Cognition.
[22] R. Ilmoniemi,et al. Brain responses reveal the learning of foreign language phonemes. , 1999, Psychophysiology.
[23] Chao He,et al. Mismatch Responses to Pitch Changes in Early Infancy , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[24] Risto Näätänen,et al. Maturation of the auditory change detection response in infants: a longitudinal ERP study , 2002, Neuroreport.
[25] S Nittrouer,et al. Developmental weighting shifts for noise components of fricative-vowel syllables. , 1997, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[26] Kanae Nishi,et al. Acoustic and perceptual similarity of Japanese and American English vowels. , 2008, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[27] C. Nelson,et al. Developmental behavioral neuroscience , 1992 .
[28] Anne Fernald,et al. How vocabulary size in two languages relates to efficiency in spoken word recognition by young Spanish–English bilinguals* , 2009, Journal of Child Language.
[29] Janet F. Werker,et al. Bilingual beginnings as a lens for theory development: PRIMIR in focus , 2011, J. Phonetics.
[30] Yan H. Yu,et al. Maturation of Speech Discrimination in 4- to 7-Yr-Old Children as Indexed by Event-Related Potential Mismatch Responses , 2010, Ear and hearing.
[31] T. Carrell,et al. Effects of lengthened formant transition duration on discrimination and neural representation of synthetic CV syllables by normal and learning-disabled children. , 1999, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[32] C. Adams,et al. Mismatch negativity and frequency representational width in children with specific language impairment , 2008, Developmental medicine and child neurology.
[33] C. Koch. The quest for consciousness : a neurobiological approach , 2004 .
[34] S. Dehaene,et al. Speed and cerebral correlates of syllable discrimination in infants , 1994, Nature.
[35] Valerie L. Shafer,et al. Neurophysiological Indexes of Speech Processing Deficits in Children with Specific Language Impairment , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[36] Megha Sundara,et al. Rhythmic distance between languages affects the development of speech perception in bilingual infants , 2011, J. Phonetics.
[37] E. Knudsen. Sensitive Periods in the Development of the Brain and Behavior , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[38] I. Winkler,et al. Processing acoustic change and novelty in newborn infants , 2007, The European journal of neuroscience.
[39] Valerie L. Shafer,et al. Electrophysiological indices of cerebral specialization and the role of prosody in language acquisition in 3‐month‐old infants , 1999 .
[40] John P. Pinto,et al. When half a word is enough: infants can recognize spoken words using partial phonetic information. , 2001, Child development.
[41] Linda Polka,et al. Development of coronal stop perception: Bilingual infants keep pace with their monolingual peers , 2008, Cognition.
[42] Mara L. Morr,et al. Maturation of Mismatch Negativity in School‐Age Children , 2000, Ear and hearing.
[43] P. Kuhl,et al. Phonetic learning as a pathway to language: new data and native language magnet theory expanded (NLM-e) , 2008, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[44] Jette G. Hansen Edwards,et al. Phonology and second language acquisition , 2008 .
[45] Michael S. Gazzaniga,et al. Interview: Mark D'Esposito with Michael S. Gazzaniga , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[46] Janet F. Werker,et al. The Roots of Bilingualism in Newborns , 2010, Psychological science.
[47] P. Jusczyk,et al. Infants' sensitivity to phonotactic patterns in the native language. , 1994 .
[48] E. Altenmüller,et al. The musician's brain as a model of neuroplasticity , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[49] A. Friederici,et al. Electrophysiological evidence for delayed mismatch response in infants at-risk for specific language impairment. , 2004, Psychophysiology.
[50] Naseem Choudhury,et al. Maturation of auditory evoked potentials from 6 to 48 months: Prediction to 3 and 4 year language and cognitive abilities , 2011, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[51] Jamie L. Metsala,et al. Young children's phonological awareness and nonword repetition as a function of vocabulary development. , 1999 .
[52] Jean K. Moore,et al. Maturation of Human Auditory Cortex: Implications for Speech Perception , 2002, The Annals of otology, rhinology & laryngology. Supplement.
[53] G. McArthur,et al. Speech and non-speech processing in people with specific language impairment: A behavioural and electrophysiological study , 2005, Brain and Language.
[54] P. Luce,et al. An examination of similarity neighbourhoods in young children's receptive vocabularies , 1995, Journal of Child Language.
[55] N. Sebastián-Gallés,et al. Simultaneous Bilingualism and the Perception of a Language-Specific Vowel Contrast in the First Year of Life , 2003, Language and speech.
[56] A. Friederici,et al. Neural manifestation of cognitive and precognitive mismatch detection in early infancy , 2002, Neuroreport.
[57] Valerie L. Shafer,et al. 6. Speech perception in second language learners: The re-education of selective perception , 2008 .
[58] Angela D Friederici,et al. Sex hormone testosterone affects language organization in the infant brain , 2008, Neuroreport.
[59] M E Miller,et al. Predicting developmental shifts in perceptual weighting schemes. , 1997, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[60] Emmanuel Dupoux,et al. Limits on bilingualism revisited: Stress ‘deafness’ in simultaneous French–Spanish bilinguals , 2010, Cognition.
[61] J. Campos,et al. Some issues pertaining to auditory evoked potentials and sex-related differences in brain development , 1982, Brain and Language.
[62] C. Koch,et al. Towards a neurobiological theory of consciousness , 1990 .
[63] Diane Kurtzberg,et al. DIFFERENTIAL MATURATION OF CORTICAL AUDITORY EVOKED POTENTIALS TO SPEECH SOUNDS IN NORMAL FULLTERM AND VERY LOW‐BIRTHWEIGHT INFANTS , 1984, Developmental medicine and child neurology.
[64] Diane Kurtzberg,et al. Language-specific memory traces of consonants in the brain. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[65] R. Näätänen,et al. The mismatch negativity (MMN) in basic research of central auditory processing: A review , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[66] P. Kuhl,et al. Brain potentials to native and non-native speech contrasts in 7- and 11-month-old American infants. , 2005, Developmental science.
[67] F. Linthicum,et al. The human auditory system: A timeline of development , 2007, International journal of audiology.
[68] J. Eggermont,et al. Maturation of human central auditory system activity: separating auditory evoked potentials by dipole source modeling , 2002, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[69] Winifred Strange,et al. Automatic selective perception (ASP) of first and second language speech: A working model , 2011, J. Phonetics.
[70] R. Uwer,et al. Automatic processing of tones and speech stimuli in children with specific language impairment. , 2002, Developmental medicine and child neurology.
[71] S. Nittrouer,et al. The effect of segmental order on fricative labeling by children and adults , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.
[72] M. Steinschneider,et al. The maturation of human evoked brain potentials to sounds presented at different stimulus rates , 2008, Hearing Research.
[73] D. Mills,et al. Two languages, one developing brain: event-related potentials to words in bilingual toddlers. , 2006, Developmental science.
[74] Bruce D. McCandliss,et al. Native Language Experience Influences the Topography of the Mismatch Negativity to Speech , 2010, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[75] N. Sebastián-Gallés,et al. Evidence of Early Language Discrimination Abilities in Infants From Bilingual Environments. , 2001, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.
[76] A. Friederici,et al. ERP correlates of processing native and non-native language word stress in infants with different language outcomes , 2009, Cortex.
[77] J. Mehler,et al. Language discrimination by newborns: toward an understanding of the role of rhythm. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[78] D. Oller,et al. Infant babbling and speech , 1976, Journal of Child Language.
[79] Robert M. Hammond,et al. The Sounds of Spanish: Analysis and Application (with Special Reference to American English) , 2001 .
[80] K. Stevens,et al. Linguistic experience alters phonetic perception in infants by 6 months of age. , 1992, Science.
[81] J. Werker,et al. Infants' Ability to Learn Phonetically Similar Words: Effects of Age and Vocabulary Size , 2002 .
[82] W. Strange,et al. Perception of a Japanese vowel length contrast by Japanese and American English listeners: Behavioral and electrophysiological measures , 2010, Brain Research.
[83] William P. Fifer,et al. Two-day-olds prefer their native language , 1993 .
[84] Erika S. Levy,et al. Perception of French vowels by American English adults with and without French language experience , 2008, J. Phonetics.
[85] Suzanne Curtin,et al. PRIMIR: A Developmental Framework of Infant Speech Processing , 2005 .
[87] Maija S. Peltola,et al. The Effect of Language Immersion Education on the Preattentive Perception of Native and Non-native Vowel Contrasts , 2007, Journal of psycholinguistic research.
[88] Massimiliano Valeriani,et al. Attentional load of the primary task influences the frontal but not the temporal generators of mismatch negativity. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[89] E. Sussman. A New View on the MMN and Attention Debate The Role of Context in Processing Auditory Events , 2007 .
[90] P. Jusczyk. The discovery of spoken language , 1997 .
[91] Helen J. Neville,et al. Language Experience and the Organization of Brain Activity to Phonetically Similar Words: ERP Evidence from 14- and 20-Month-Olds , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[92] Todd M. Bailey,et al. Phonological specificity in early words , 2002 .
[93] I. Knopf. A Decade Later: A Follow-up of Social Class and Mental Illness. , 1968 .
[94] Mara L. Morr,et al. Neurophysiological indices of attention to speech in children with specific language impairment , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[95] Elizabeth Hughes,et al. Proceedings of the 21th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development , 1997 .