Phase shifting by novelty-induced running: activity dose-response curves at different circadian times

Abstract This study compared phase shifting after novelty-induced running at different circadian times (CTs). In Experiment 1, hamsters were confined to novel wheels for 3 h, starting at CTs 2, 4, 6, 8, 10 or 22. The largest shifts were found at CTs 2, 4 and 6. At each CT there was a relationship between the number of revolutions during the pulse and the size of phase shift. Maximum shifts were usually observed at each CT when animals ran 5000–9000 revolutions during the pulse. In Experiment 2, hamsters were confined to novel wheels for 1 h, also starting at CTs 2, 4, 6, 8, 10 or 22. Unlike with 3-h pulses, the largest shifts with 1-h pulses occurred at CT 8. In Experiment 3, hamsters were shut into a small nest box after a 1-h pulse at CT 8; phase shifting was unaffected, showing that movement about the home cage after a 1-h pulse had ended was not required for shifting. At CTs 2, 4 and 22, 3-h pulses produced shifts but 1-h pulses did not. Possibly, there are two different mechanisms of nonphotic phase shifting that can be activated by being placed in a novel wheel, but the results can also be explained in terms of a single mechanism.

[1]  N. Mrosovsky,et al.  Neuropeptide y and behaviorally induced phase shifts , 1994, Neuroscience.

[2]  N. Mrosovsky,et al.  Nonphotically induced phase shifts of circadian rhythms in the golden hamster: Activity-response curves at different ambient temperatures , 1993, Physiology & Behavior.

[3]  N. Mrosovsky,et al.  Triazolam and phase-shifting acceleration re-evaluated. , 1990, Chronobiology international.

[4]  N. Mrosovsky Methods of measuring phase shifts: why I continue to use an Aschoff type II procedure despite the skepticism of referees. , 1996, Chronobiology international.

[5]  N. Mrosovsky,et al.  Effects of Induced Wheel Running on the Circadian Activity Rhythms of Syrian Hamsters: Entrainment and Phase Response Curve , 1989, Journal of biological rhythms.

[6]  J. Takahashi,et al.  Sensitivity and integration in a visual pathway for circadian entrainment in the hamster (Mesocricetus auratus). , 1991, The Journal of physiology.

[7]  R. Mistlberger,et al.  Morphine phase-shifts circadian rhythms in mice: role of behavioural activation , 1995, Neuroreport.

[8]  H. Albers,et al.  Neuropeptide Y microinjected into the suprachiasmatic region phase shifts circadian rhythms in constant darkness , 1994, Peptides.

[9]  N. Mrosovsky,et al.  Lengthening of Circadian Period in Hamsters by Novelty-Induced Wheel Running , 1997, Physiology & Behavior.

[10]  F. Turek,et al.  Stimulated activity mediates phase shifts in the hamster circadian clock induced by dark pulses or benzodiazepines , 1989, Nature.

[11]  N. Mrosovsky,et al.  Circadian phase-shifts induced by chlordiazepoxide without increased locomotor activity , 1993, Brain Research.

[12]  F. Turek,et al.  Phase-shifting effects of acute increases in activity on circadian locomotor rhythms in hamsters. , 1991, The American journal of physiology.

[13]  N. Mrosovsky,et al.  Serotonergic stimulation and nonphotic phase-shifting in hamsters , 1996, Physiology & Behavior.

[14]  E. Maywood,et al.  Non-photic phase shifting of the circadian activity rhythm of Syrian hamsters: the relative potency of arousal and melatonin , 1992, Brain Research.

[15]  F. James Rohlf,et al.  Biometry: The Principles and Practice of Statistics in Biological Research , 1969 .

[16]  N. Mrosovsky,et al.  Nonphotic Phase Shifting in Hamster Clock Mutants , 1992, Journal of biological rhythms.