Temporal discrimination of one’s own reaction times in dual-task performance: Context effects and methodological constraints

In this study we used the method of constant stimuli to investigate introspective reaction times in the psychological refractory period (PRP) paradigm under different temporal contexts. Previous introspective PRP studies have mostly used visual analogue scales to assess introspective reaction times and found that participants were largely unaware of the typical dual-task costs that arise in this paradigm (PRP effect). This apparent limitation of introspection has been taken as evidence for a serial processing bottleneck that encompasses response selection as well as conscious perception. In our study, in each trial participants first performed the PRP task and were then presented with a comparison interval that they had to compare with their reaction time to the second task (RT2). Across three experiments, we observed that the subjective estimates of RT2 (i.e., the points of subjective equality) did not reflect the objective pattern but were almost completely biased toward the center of the comparison intervals (asymmetry effect). In a control experiment in which participants discriminated RT2s of other participants without performing the PRP task, this bias was largely reduced. We interpret these results as indicating that in dual-task performance participants acquire only poor temporal representations of their own reaction times, and the apparent unawareness of the PRP effect may reflect disturbed timing abilities rather than a conscious perception bottleneck.

[1]  Andrea Kiesel,et al.  Assessing intentional binding with the method of constant stimuli , 2012, Consciousness and Cognition.

[2]  F. Restle,et al.  Invalid results from the method of constant stimuli , 1968 .

[3]  K. Danziger The history of introspection reconsidered. , 1980 .

[4]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Limits on Introspection Distorted Subjective Time During the Dual-Task Bottleneck , 2008 .

[5]  W. Matthews Stimulus Repetition and the Perception of Time: The Effects of Prior Exposure on Temporal Discrimination, Judgment, and Production , 2011, PloS one.

[6]  R. Ulrich,et al.  Trial-by-trial updating of an internal reference in discrimination tasks: Evidence from effects of stimulus order and trial sequence , 2012, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.

[7]  Daniel Bratzke,et al.  Introspective reports of reaction times in dual-tasks reflect experienced difficulty rather than timing of cognitive processes , 2014, Consciousness and Cognition.

[8]  Harold Pashler,et al.  Mental timing and the central attentional bottleneck , 2010 .

[9]  Vincent Walsh,et al.  Memory for Time Distinguishes between Perception and Action , 2010, Perception.

[10]  N. Taatgen,et al.  Timing of multiple overlapping intervals: how many clocks do we have? , 2008, Acta psychologica.

[11]  R. Ulrich,et al.  Does the asymmetry effect inflate the temporal expansion of odd stimuli? , 2010, Psychological research.

[12]  Doughty Jm The effect of psychophysical method and context on pitch and loudness functions. , 1949 .

[13]  R. Block,et al.  Prospective and retrospective duration judgments: A meta-analytic review , 1997, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[14]  Response Time to a First Signal as a Function of Time Relationship to a Second Signal and Mode of Presentation , 1971, Perceptual and motor skills.

[15]  Rolf Ulrich,et al.  Crossmodal temporal discrimination: Assessing the predictions of a general pacemaker-counter model , 2006, Perception & psychophysics.

[16]  Scott W. Brown Attentional resources in timing: Interference effects in concurrent temporal and nontemporal working memory tasks , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.

[17]  Warren H. Meck,et al.  Bayesian optimization of time perception , 2013, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[18]  Daniel Bratzke,et al.  Distorted subjective reports of stimulus onsets under dual-task conditions: Delayed conscious perception or estimation bias? , 2014, Consciousness and Cognition.

[19]  Helga Lejeune,et al.  Vierordt's The Experimental Study of the Time Sense (1868) and its legacy , 2009 .

[20]  D. Bryce,et al.  How are overlapping time intervals perceived? Evidence for a weighted sum of segments model. , 2015, Acta psychologica.

[21]  P. Hancock,et al.  How cognitive load affects duration judgments: A meta-analytic review. , 2010, Acta psychologica.

[22]  E. Boring A history of introspection. , 1953, Psychological bulletin.

[23]  Daniel Bratzke,et al.  Are introspective reaction times affected by the method of time estimation? A comparison of visual analogue scales and reproduction , 2014, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.

[24]  Christopher L. Asplund,et al.  A Unified attentional bottleneck in the human brain , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[25]  Richard D. Morey,et al.  Confidence Intervals from Normalized Data: A correction to Cousineau (2005) , 2008 .

[26]  S. C. Masin Different biases in the methods of constant and single stimuli , 1987 .

[27]  Martin Riemer Psychophysics and the anisotropy of time , 2015, Consciousness and Cognition.

[28]  Simon Grondin,et al.  Duration discrimination of empty and filled intervals marked by auditory and visual signals , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[29]  S. Grondin,et al.  Different methods for reproducing time, different results , 2014, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.

[30]  R. Ulrich,et al.  Duration perception of visual and auditory oddball stimuli: Does judgment task modulate the temporal oddball effect? , 2014, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.

[31]  Multiple timing of nested intervals: Further evidence for a weighted sum of segments account , 2016, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[32]  Simon Grondin,et al.  An Ecological Approach to Prospective and Retrospective Timing of Long Durations: A Study Involving Gamers , 2010, PloS one.

[33]  Mariano Sigman,et al.  A shared cortical bottleneck underlying Attentional Blink and Psychological Refractory Period , 2012, NeuroImage.

[34]  J. Devin McAuley,et al.  Time judgments in global temporal contexts , 2005, Perception & psychophysics.

[35]  P Bertelson,et al.  The refractory period of choice reactions with regular and irregular interstimuli intervals. , 1967, Acta psychologica.

[36]  J. Changeux,et al.  Opinion TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.10 No.5 May 2006 Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: a testable taxonomy , 2022 .

[37]  E. Poulton Models for biases in judging sensory magnitude. , 1979, Psychological bulletin.

[38]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Mapping introspection’s blind spot: Reconstruction of dual-task phenomenology using quantified introspection , 2010, Cognition.

[39]  Rolf Ulrich,et al.  Formation and representation of temporal reference information , 2016, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences.

[40]  A. J. Sanford,et al.  Rating the speed of a simple reaction , 1970 .

[41]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[42]  P. Tse,et al.  Attention and the subjective expansion of time , 2004, Perception & psychophysics.