Vision adds to haptics when dyads perform a whole-body joint balance task
暂无分享,去创建一个
Karen Zentgraf | Rouwen Cañal-Bruland | Eric Eils | Marc H E de Lussanet | Leonie Sieverding | R. Cañal-Bruland | M. D. de Lussanet | K. Zentgraf | E. Eils | Leonie Sieverding
[1] 付伶俐. 打磨Using Language,倡导新理念 , 2014 .
[2] Michael J. Richardson,et al. Dynamics of Interpersonal Coordination , 2008 .
[3] N. Sebanz,et al. Our actions in my mind: Motor imagery of joint action , 2014, Neuropsychologia.
[4] M. van der Schoot,et al. The developmental onset of symbolic approximation: beyond nonsymbolic representations, the language of numbers matters , 2015, Front. Psychol..
[5] Kristin L. Sainani,et al. Bonferroni, Holm, and Hochberg Corrections: Fun Names, Serious Changes to P Values , 2014, PM & R : the journal of injury, function, and rehabilitation.
[6] N. Sebanz,et al. Psychological research on joint action: Theory and data , 2011 .
[7] U. Castiello,et al. Does the intention to communicate affect action kinematics? , 2009, Consciousness and Cognition.
[8] Karen Zentgraf,et al. Simulation during observation of human actions – Theories, empirical studies, applications , 2011, Vision Research.
[9] A. Wing,et al. Optimal feedback correction in string quartet synchronization , 2014, Journal of The Royal Society Interface.
[10] Marco Santello,et al. On the Role of Physical Interaction on Performance of Object Manipulation by Dyads , 2017, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[11] A. Mizuno,et al. A change of the leading player in flow Visualization technique , 2006, J. Vis..
[12] G. Pezzulo,et al. Human Sensorimotor Communication: A Theory of Signaling in Online Social Interactions , 2013, PloS one.
[13] Robrecht P R D van der Wel,et al. Let the force be with us: dyads exploit haptic coupling for coordination. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[14] Sebastian Wallot,et al. Beyond Synchrony: Joint Action in a Complex Production Task Reveals Beneficial Effects of Decreased Interpersonal Synchrony , 2016, PloS one.
[15] Michael J. Richardson,et al. Strategic communication and behavioral coupling in asymmetric joint action , 2014, Experimental Brain Research.
[16] Karl J. Friston,et al. The pragmatic turn : toward action-oriented views in cognitive science , 2016 .
[17] Jörn Diedrichsen,et al. Integration of vision and haptics during tool use. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[18] H. Bekkering,et al. Joint action: bodies and minds moving together , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[19] M. Ernst,et al. Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion , 2002, Nature.
[20] Alan M. Wing,et al. Perception of string quartet synchronization , 2014, Front. Psychol..
[21] C. Frith,et al. Follow you, Follow me: Continuous Mutual Prediction and Adaptation in Joint Tapping , 2010, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[22] H. H. Clark,et al. Speaking while monitoring addressees for understanding , 2004 .
[23] Giovanni Pezzulo,et al. Intentional strategies that make co-actors more predictable: the case of signaling. , 2013 .
[24] Jurjen Bosga,et al. Intra- and interpersonal movement coordination in jointly moving a rocking board. , 2010, Motor control.
[25] Christopher A. Dickinson,et al. Coordinating cognition: The costs and benefits of shared gaze during collaborative search , 2008, Cognition.
[26] Barbara Anne Dosher,et al. Task precision at transfer determines specificity of perceptual learning. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[27] C. Gonzalez,et al. The contributions of vision and haptics to reaching and grasping , 2015, Front. Psychol..
[28] A. Roepstorff,et al. The two-brain approach: how can mutually interacting brains teach us something about social interaction? , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[29] C. Palmer,et al. Synchronization of Timing and Motion 435 , 2022 .
[30] Kevin Shockley,et al. Joint action in a cooperative precision task: nested processes of intrapersonal and interpersonal coordination , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.
[31] Robyn Carston. Herbert H. Clark, Using language . Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1996. Pp. xi+432. , 1999 .
[32] K. Vogeley,et al. Toward a second-person neuroscience 1 , 2013, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[33] Michael J. Richardson,et al. Rocking together: dynamics of intentional and unintentional interpersonal coordination. , 2007, Human movement science.
[34] M. Candidi,et al. Kinematics fingerprints of leader and follower role-taking during cooperative joint actions , 2013, Experimental Brain Research.
[35] Vassilia Hatzitaki,et al. Somatosensory driven interpersonal synchrony during rhythmic sway. , 2012, Human movement science.
[36] Ryan Ferguson,et al. Consequences of joint action: Entanglement with your partner. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[37] R. E Passingham,et al. Inferring false beliefs from the actions of oneself and others: an fMRI study , 2004, NeuroImage.
[38] Cordula Vesper,et al. Making oneself predictable: reduced temporal variability facilitates joint action coordination , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.
[39] W. Prinz,et al. Perceptual resonance: action-induced modulation of perception , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[40] N. Sebanz,et al. The role of shared visual information for joint action coordination , 2016, Cognition.
[41] Lars Kai. Frontal alpha oscillations distinguish leaders from followers: Multivariate decoding of mutually interacting brains , 2016 .
[42] J. Decety,et al. From the perception of action to the understanding of intention , 2001, Nature reviews. Neuroscience.
[43] Lucia M. Sacheli,et al. Social cues to joint actions: the role of shared goals , 2015, Front. Psychol..
[44] Cordula Vesper,et al. A minimal architecture for joint action , 2010, Neural Networks.