Using video images to improve the accuracy of surrogate decision-making: a randomized controlled trial.

INTRODUCTION When patients are unable to make important end-of-life decisions, doctors ask surrogate decision makers to provide insight into patients' preferences. Unfortunately, multiple studies have shown that surrogates' knowledge of patient preferences is poor. We hypothesized that a video decision tool would improve concordance between patients and their surrogates for end-of-life preferences. OBJECTIVE To compare the concordance of preferences among elderly patients and their surrogates listening to only a verbal description of advanced dementia or viewing a video decision support tool of the disease after hearing the verbal description. METHODS This was a randomized controlled trial of a convenience sample of community-dwelling elderly subjects (> or = 65 years) and their surrogates, and was conducted at 2 geriatric clinics affiliated with 2 academic medical centers in Boston. The study was conducted between September 1, 2007, and May 30, 2008. Random assignment of patient and surrogate dyads was to either a verbal narrative or a video decision support tool after the verbal narrative. End points were goals of care chosen by the patient and predicted goals of care by the surrogate. Goals of care included life-prolonging care (CPR, mechanical ventilation), limited care (hospitalization, antibiotics, but not CPR), and comfort care (only treatment to relieve symptoms). The primary outcome measure was the concordance rate of preferences between patients and their surrogates. RESULTS A total of 14 pairs of patients and their surrogates were randomized to verbal narrative (n=6) or video after verbal narrative (n=8). Among the 6 patients receiving only the verbal narrative, 3 (50%) preferred comfort care, 1 (17%) chose limited care, and 2 (33%) desired life-prolonging care. Among the surrogates for these patients, only 2 correctly chose what their loved one would want if in a state of advanced dementia, yielding a concordance rate of 33%. Among the 8 patients receiving the video decision support tool, all 8 chose comfort care. Among the surrogates for these patients, all 8 correctly chose what their loved one would want if in a state of advanced dementia, yielding a concordance rate of 100%. CONCLUSION Patients and surrogates viewing a video decision support tool for advanced dementia are more likely to concur about the patient's end-of-life preferences than when solely listening to a verbal description of the disease.

[1]  C. Cassel,et al.  Treatment choices at the end of life: a comparison of decisions by older patients and their physician-selected proxies. , 1989, The Gerontologist.

[2]  S. Shaykevich,et al.  Using video images of dementia in advance care planning. , 2007, Archives of internal medicine.

[3]  D. Sulmasy,et al.  A prospective trial of a new policy eliminating signed consent for do not resuscitate orders , 2006, Journal of General Internal Medicine.

[4]  T. Beauchamp,et al.  Principles of biomedical ethics , 1991 .

[5]  W. Knaus,et al.  Do Formal Advance Directives Affect Resuscitation Decisions and the Use of Resources for Seriously Ill Patients? , 1994, The Journal of Clinical Ethics.

[6]  V. Tilden,et al.  Family decision-making to withdraw life-sustaining treatments from hospitalized patients. , 2001, Nursing research.

[7]  B. Reisberg Functional assessment staging (FAST). , 1988, Psychopharmacology bulletin.

[8]  J. Lynn,et al.  Patient Preferences for Communication with Physicians about End-of-Life Decisions , 1997, Annals of Internal Medicine.

[9]  M. Slaughter Documenting the Documentary: Close Readings of Documentary Film and Video , 1998 .

[10]  K. Covinsky,et al.  Communication and Decision‐Making in Seriously Ill Patients: Findings of the SUPPORT Project , 2000, Journal of the American Geriatrics Society.

[11]  L. Forrow,et al.  Deciding for others: The ethics of surrogate decision‐making , 1990 .

[12]  C. Anderson The Psychology of Doing Nothing: Forms of Decision Avoidance Result from Reason and Emotion , 2003, Psychological bulletin.

[13]  D. Brock What is the moral authority of family members to act as surrogates for incompetent patients? , 1996, The Milbank quarterly.

[14]  E. Pfeiffer A Short Portable Mental Status Questionnaire for the Assessment of Organic Brain Deficit in Elderly Patients † , 1975, Journal of the American Geriatrics Society.

[15]  William A. Knaus,et al.  A controlled trial to improve care for seriously ill hospitalized patients. The study to understand prognoses and preferences for outcomes and risks of treatments (SUPPORT). The SUPPORT Principal Investigators. , 1995, JAMA.

[16]  M. Barry,et al.  Video decision support tool for advance care planning in dementia: randomised controlled trial , 2009, BMJ : British Medical Journal.

[17]  J. Cornell,et al.  Medical Treatment Preferences of Nursing Home Residents: Relationship to Function and Concordance with Surrogate Decision‐Makers , 1993, Journal of the American Geriatrics Society.

[18]  Shimon Shaykevich,et al.  Health literacy not race predicts end-of-life care preferences. , 2008, Journal of palliative medicine.

[19]  B. Reisberg,et al.  Functional Assessment Staging (FAST) in Alzheimer's Disease: Reliability, Validity, and Ordinality , 1992, International Psychogeriatrics.

[20]  D. Meier,et al.  Substituted judgment: how accurate are proxy predictions? , 1991, Annals of internal medicine.

[21]  Robert M Arnold,et al.  Moral justifications for surrogate decision making in the intensive care unit: implications and limitations. , 2003, Critical care medicine.

[22]  T. Garrick,et al.  Myth of substituted judgment. Surrogate decision making regarding life support is unreliable. , 1994, Archives of internal medicine.

[23]  J. Lynn,et al.  Surrogates' predictions of seriously ill patients' resuscitation preferences. , 1995, Archives of family medicine.

[24]  M. Paasche-Orlow,et al.  Overcoming educational barriers for advance care planning in Latinos with video images. , 2008, Journal of palliative medicine.

[25]  E. Arkin,et al.  Making Health Communication Programs Work. A Planner's Guide. , 1989 .