The role of the striatum and hippocampus in planning: a PET activation study in Parkinson's disease.

Previous work has identified the prefrontal cortex (PFC) and striatum as participating in the planning and selection of movements. We compared the brain activation patterns during planning in Parkinson's disease patients and age-matched controls using H(2)(15)O-PET and the Tower of London (TOL) task. In this study, our mildly affected Parkinson's disease group performed as well as the control group but showed a different pattern of neuronal activation. In the two groups, overlapping areas of the PFC were activated but, whereas the right caudate nucleus was activated in the control group, this was not evident in the Parkinson's disease patients. This suggests that normal normal frontal lobe activation can occur in Parkinson's disease despite abnormal processing within the basal ganglia. Moreover, right hippocampus activity was suppressed in the controls and enhanced in the Parkinson's disease patients. This could represent a shift to the declarative memory system in Parkinson's disease during performance of the TOL task, possibly resulting from insufficient working memory capacity within the frontostriatal system.

[1]  M. Hoehn,et al.  Parkinsonism , 1967, Neurology.

[2]  Suzanne Corkin,et al.  ACQUISITION OF MOTOR SKILL AFTER BILATERAL MEDIAL TEMPORAL-LOBE EXCISION , 1968 .

[3]  T. Shallice Specific impairments of planning. , 1982, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[4]  Y. Agid,et al.  Reduction of cortical dopamine, noradrenaline, serotonin and their metabolites in Parkinson's disease , 1983, Brain Research.

[5]  G. Lynch,et al.  Hippocampal denervation causes rapid forgetting of olfactory information in rats. , 1984, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[6]  K. Flowers,et al.  The effect of Parkinson's disease on the ability to maintain a mental set. , 1985, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[7]  H Eichenbaum,et al.  Normal olfactory discrimination learning set and facilitation of reversal learning after medial-temporal damage in rats: implications for an account of preserved learning abilities in amnesia , 1986, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[8]  G. E. Alexander,et al.  Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. , 1986, Annual review of neuroscience.

[9]  S J Kish,et al.  Biochemical pathophysiology of Parkinson's disease. , 1987, Advances in neurology.

[10]  T. Robbins,et al.  Planning and spatial working memory in Parkinson's disease. , 1988, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[11]  T. H. Burnstine,et al.  Movement Disorders 2 , 1988, Neurology.

[12]  J. Talairach,et al.  Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain: 3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging , 1988 .

[13]  Wade K. Smith,et al.  Midbrain dopaminergic cell loss in parkinson's disease: Computer visualization , 1989, Annals of neurology.

[14]  M. Packard,et al.  Differential effects of fornix and caudate nucleus lesions on two radial maze tasks: evidence for multiple memory systems , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[15]  J. Penney,et al.  The functional anatomy of basal ganglia disorders , 1989, Trends in Neurosciences.

[16]  A. C. Roberts,et al.  Impaired extra-dimensional shift performance in medicated and unmedicated Parkinson's disease: Evidence for a specific attentional dysfunction , 1989, Neuropsychologia.

[17]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  The Relationship between Global and Local Changes in PET Scans , 1990, Journal of cerebral blood flow and metabolism : official journal of the International Society of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism.

[18]  E. Marder,et al.  Procedural memory in Parkinson's disease: impaired motor but not visuoperceptual learning. , 1990, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[19]  K. Flowers,et al.  Motor set in Parkinson's disease. , 1990, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[20]  M. Posner,et al.  The attention system of the human brain. , 1990, Annual review of neuroscience.

[21]  T. Robbins,et al.  Planning and spatial working memory following frontal lobe lesions in man , 1990, Neuropsychologia.

[22]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Topographic segregation of corticostriatal projections from posterior parietal subdivisions in the macaque monkey , 1991, Neuroscience.

[23]  D. Pandya,et al.  Prefrontostriatal connections in relation to cortical architectonic organization in rhesus monkeys , 1991, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[24]  E. Sullivan,et al.  Cognitive impairment in early, untreated Parkinson's disease and its relationship to motor disability. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[25]  W Fernandez,et al.  Impaired activation of the supplementary motor area in Parkinson's disease is reversed when akinesia is treated with apomorphine , 1992, Annals of neurology.

[26]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Impaired mesial frontal and putamen activation in Parkinson's disease: A positron emission tomography study , 1992, Annals of neurology.

[27]  C. Marsden,et al.  Fronto-striatal cognitive deficits at different stages of Parkinson's disease. , 1992, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[28]  M. Reivich,et al.  Attention and sentence processing deficits in Parkinson's disease: the role of anterior cingulate cortex. , 1992, Cerebral cortex.

[29]  T J Spinks,et al.  Physical performance of a positron tomograph for brain imaging with retractable septa. , 1992, Physics in medicine and biology.

[30]  J. Aggleton,et al.  The effects of fornix and medial prefrontal lesions on delayed non-matching-to-sample by rats , 1993, Behavioural Brain Research.

[31]  R. J. McDonald,et al.  A triple dissociation of memory systems: hippocampus, amygdala, and dorsal striatum. , 1993, Behavioral neuroscience.

[32]  T. Robbins,et al.  Contrasting mechanisms of impaired attentional set-shifting in patients with frontal lobe damage or Parkinson's disease. , 1993, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[33]  D. Pandya,et al.  Striatal connections of the parietal association cortices in rhesus monkeys , 1993, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[34]  D. Balota,et al.  Implicit Memory and the Formation of New Associations in Nondemented Parkinson′s Disease Individuals and Individuals with Senile Dementia of the Alzheimer Type: A Serial Reaction Time (SRT) Investigation , 1993, Brain and Cognition.

[35]  R. Passingham The frontal lobes and voluntary action , 1993 .

[36]  M. Delong,et al.  Pathophysiology of parkinsonian motor abnormalities. , 1993, Advances in neurology.

[37]  S. Haber,et al.  Primate cingulostriatal projection: Limbic striatal versus sensorimotor striatal input , 1994, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[38]  M. Petrides Comparative architectonic analysis of the human and the macaque frontal cortex , 1994 .

[39]  E. Rolls Neurophysiology and cognitive functions of the striatum. , 1994, Revue neurologique.

[40]  P. Strick,et al.  Anatomical evidence for cerebellar and basal ganglia involvement in higher cognitive function. , 1994, Science.

[41]  Joel L. Davis,et al.  Toward a Circuit Model of Working Memory and the Guidance of Voluntary Motor Action , 1994 .

[42]  Adrian M. Owen,et al.  Visuo-spatial short-term recognition memory and learning after temporal lobe excisions, frontal lobe excisions or amygdalo-hippocampectomy in man , 1995, Neuropsychologia.

[43]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Pallidotomy increases activity of motor association cortex in parkinson's disease: A positron emission tomographic study , 1995, Annals of neurology.

[44]  Patricia S. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Toward a circuit model of working memory and the guidance of voluntary motor action , 1995 .

[45]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Spatial registration and normalization of images , 1995 .

[46]  M Moscovitch,et al.  Recovered consciousness: a hypothesis concerning modularity and episodic memory. , 1995, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[47]  C. Marsden,et al.  Self-initiated versus externally triggered movements. I. An investigation using measurement of regional cerebral blood flow with PET and movement-related potentials in normal and Parkinson's disease subjects. , 1995, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[48]  Joel L. Davis,et al.  Adaptive Critics and the Basal Ganglia , 1995 .

[49]  P. Strick,et al.  Motor areas of the medial wall: a review of their location and functional activation. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[50]  M. Hallett,et al.  Cerebral structures participating in motor preparation in humans: a positron emission tomography study. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[51]  L. Squire,et al.  Structure and function of declarative and nondeclarative memory systems. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[52]  Jennifer A. Mangels,et al.  A Neostriatal Habit Learning System in Humans , 1996, Science.

[53]  J. D. McGaugh,et al.  Inactivation of Hippocampus or Caudate Nucleus with Lidocaine Differentially Affects Expression of Place and Response Learning , 1996, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[54]  R. Dolan,et al.  Active representation of shape and spatial location in man. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[55]  R. Dolan,et al.  Neural systems engaged by planning: a PET study of the Tower of London task , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[56]  C. Gerfen,et al.  The frontal cortex-basal ganglia system in primates. , 1996, Critical reviews in neurobiology.

[57]  R. Passingham,et al.  Self-initiated versus externally triggered movements. I. An investigation using measurement of regional cerebral blood flow with PET and movement-related potentials in normal and Parkinson's disease subjects. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[58]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Planning and Spatial Working Memory: a Positron Emission Tomography Study in Humans , 1996, The European journal of neuroscience.

[59]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Functional Anatomy of Visuomotor Skill Learning in Human Subjects Examined with Positron Emission Tomography , 1996, The European journal of neuroscience.

[60]  A M Owen,et al.  Double dissociations of memory and executive functions in working memory tasks following frontal lobe excisions, temporal lobe excisions or amygdalo-hippocampectomy in man. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[61]  N. White Mnemonic functions of the basal ganglia , 1997, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[62]  T. Robbins,et al.  Spatial and non-spatial working memory at different stages of Parkinson's disease , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[63]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Regional Cerebral Blood Flow Changes as a Function of Delta and Spindle Activity during Slow Wave Sleep in Humans , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[64]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Changes in cerebral activity pattern due to subthalamic nucleus or internal pallidum stimulation in Parkinson's disease , 1997, Annals of neurology.

[65]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Cognitive Conjunction: A New Approach to Brain Activation Experiments , 1997, NeuroImage.

[66]  A. Lees,et al.  The relationship between dementia and direct involvement of the hippocampus and amygdala in Parkinson's disease , 1997, Neurology.

[67]  S. Rauch,et al.  Striatal recruitment during an implicit sequence learning task as measured by functional magnetic resonance imaging , 1997, Human brain mapping.

[68]  N P Quinn,et al.  Pallidotomy in Parkinson's disease increases supplementary motor area and prefrontal activation during performance of volitional movements an H2(15)O PET study. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[69]  T. Robbins,et al.  Frontal-striatal cognitive deficits in patients with chronic schizophrenia. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[70]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance. , 1998, Science.

[71]  T. Robbins,et al.  Dissociating executive mechanisms of task control following frontal lobe damage and Parkinson's disease. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[72]  E. Tulving,et al.  Hippocampal PET activations of memory encoding and retrieval: The HIPER model , 1998, Hippocampus.

[73]  E. Kandel,et al.  Cognitive Neuroscience and the Study of Memory , 1998, Neuron.

[74]  T. Paus,et al.  Regional differences in the effects of task difficulty and motor output on blood flow response in the human anterior cingulate cortex: a review of 107 PET activation studies , 1998, Neuroreport.

[75]  T. Klingberg Concurrent performance of two working memory tasks: potential mechanisms of interference. , 1998, Cerebral cortex.

[76]  J. Houk,et al.  Model of cortical-basal ganglionic processing: encoding the serial order of sensory events. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[77]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Abnormal basal ganglia outflow in Parkinson's disease identified with PET. Implications for higher cortical functions. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[78]  D. Schacter,et al.  Medial temporal lobe activations in fMRI and PET studies of episodic encoding and retrieval , 1999, Hippocampus.

[79]  R. Grossman,et al.  Neuropsychological outcome following unilateral pallidotomy. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[80]  R. Coppola,et al.  Physiological characteristics of capacity constraints in working memory as revealed by functional MRI. , 1999, Cerebral cortex.

[81]  Alex Martin,et al.  Automatic activation of the medial temporal lobe during encoding: Lateralized influences of meaning and novelty , 1999, Hippocampus.

[82]  Carol A. Seger,et al.  Striatal activation during acquisition of a cognitive skill. , 1999, Neuropsychology.

[83]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Frontal, midbrain and striatal dopaminergic function in early and advanced Parkinson's disease A 3D [(18)F]dopa-PET study. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[84]  A. Dagher,et al.  Mapping the network for planning: a correlational PET activation study with the Tower of London task. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[85]  R. Passingham,et al.  Self-initiated versus externally triggered movements. II. The effect of movement predictability on regional cerebral blood flow. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[86]  T. Robbins,et al.  Contrasting Cortical and Subcortical Activations Produced by Attentional-Set Shifting and Reversal Learning in Humans , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[87]  A. Lang,et al.  Long-term follow-up of unilateral pallidotomy in advanced Parkinson's disease. , 2000, The New England journal of medicine.

[88]  John D. E. Gabrieli,et al.  Impaired Frontostriatal Cognitive Functioning Following Posteroventral Pallidotomy in Advanced Parkinson's Disease , 2000, Brain and Cognition.

[89]  Anthony E. Lang,et al.  Neuropsychological Outcome of GPi Pallidotomy and GPi or STN Deep Brain Stimulation in Parkinson's Disease , 2000, Brain and Cognition.

[90]  R E Gross,et al.  Relationship of lesion location to cognitive outcome following microelectrode-guided pallidotomy for Parkinson's disease: support for the existence of cognitive circuits in the human pallidum. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.