The categorical representation of visual pattern information by young infants

Abstract The present research studied 3- and 4-month-old infants' ability to acquire two categories simultaneously. A familiarization-novelty preference procedure and geometrical form categories were used in all experiments. In Experiments I and 3, infants were familiarized with either a single form category, two form categories, or a single form category plus a set of forms that did not define a category. The results showed that, despite increased attentional and memorial demands, presentation of an additional form category did not harm the efficiency of categorization (Experiment 1) and changed the representation of the form category information from exemplars to a prototype (Experiment 3). Contrasting form information that was not categorical in structure decreased the infant's ability to recognize new members of the single familiar category (Experiment 1) and hindered the infant's ability to form a categorical representation (Experiment 3). The categorization behavior observed in Experiment 1, as indexed by the generalization of habituation to novel forms from a familiar category, was shown not to be a consequence of the inability to discriminate between individual members from the familiar form category (Experiment 2). The implications of these results for cognitive development are discussed.

[1]  M. Posner,et al.  On the genesis of abstract ideas. , 1968, Journal of experimental psychology.

[2]  J. Fagan Memory in the infant. , 1970, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[3]  A. J. Caron,et al.  Degree of stimulus complexity and habituation of visual fixation in infants , 1969 .

[4]  T. Sherman Categorization skills in infants. , 1985, Child development.

[5]  J.A. Anderson,et al.  Theory of categorization based on distributed memory storage. , 1984 .

[6]  S. Engel Thought and Language , 1964 .

[7]  J. L. Myers Fundamentals of Experimental Design , 1972 .

[8]  P. Quinn,et al.  Evidence for a general category of oblique orientations in four-month-old infants. , 1986, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[9]  P. D. Eimas,et al.  The Relation between Identification and Discrimination along Speech and Non-Speech Continua , 1963 .

[10]  P. C. Bomba,et al.  The development of orientation categories between 2 and 4 months of age. , 1984, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[11]  L. Cohen,et al.  Concept acquisition in the human infant. , 1979, Child development.

[12]  D. Pisoni,et al.  Reaction times to comparisons within and across phonetic categories , 1974, Perception & psychophysics.

[13]  E. Rosch,et al.  Categorization of Natural Objects , 1981 .

[14]  V. R. Carlson,et al.  Infant perception of the invariant shape of objects varying in slant. , 1979, Child development.

[15]  J. Bransford,et al.  Abstraction of visual patterns. , 1971, Journal of experimental psychology.

[16]  P. D. Eimas,et al.  chapter 6 – Speech Perception in Early Infancy1 , 1975 .

[17]  M. Strauss,et al.  Abstraction of prototypical information by adults and 10-month-old infants. , 1979, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.

[18]  P. Kuhl Speech perception in early infancy: perceptual constancy for spectrally dissimilar vowel categories. , 1979, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[19]  E. Gibson Principles of Perceptual Learning and Development , 1969 .

[20]  D. Homa Prototype abstraction and classification of new instances as a function of number of instances defining the prototype , 1973 .

[21]  B. C. Griffith,et al.  The discrimination of speech sounds within and across phoneme boundaries. , 1957, Journal of experimental psychology.

[22]  Eleanor Rosch,et al.  Principles of Categorization , 1978 .

[23]  Leslie B. Cohen,et al.  Infant Learning of Ill-Defined Categories. , 1981 .

[24]  P. D. Eimas,et al.  Speech Perception in Infants , 1971, Science.

[25]  Charles G. Gross,et al.  Perceptual similarity of mirror images in infancy , 1978, Cognition.

[26]  A. E. Milewski,et al.  Visual Discrimination and Detection of Configurational Invariance in 3-Month Infants. , 1979 .

[27]  E. R. Siqueland,et al.  The nature and structure of infant form categories , 1983 .

[28]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Distributed memory and the representation of general and specific information. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[29]  R. L. Fantz Visual Experience in Infants: Decreased Attention to Familiar Patterns Relative to Novel Ones , 1964, Science.

[30]  P. D. Eimas,et al.  Studies on the categorization of speech by infants , 1983, Cognition.

[31]  M. Bornstein,et al.  Color vision and hue categorization in young human infants. , 1976, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[32]  C. L. Hull Quantitative aspects of evolution of concepts: An experimental study. , 1920 .

[33]  Daniel F. Chambliss,et al.  The relative contributions of common and distinctive information on the abstraction from ill-defined categories , 1975 .

[34]  Paul C. Quinn,et al.  On Categorization in Early Infancy. , 1986 .

[35]  A. J. Caron,et al.  The effects of repeated exposure and stimulus complexity on visual fixation in infants , 1968 .

[36]  E. R. Siqueland,et al.  Delayed recognition memory for orientation by human infants. , 1985, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[37]  J. Bruner,et al.  Studies In Cognitive Growth , 1966 .

[38]  R. Lasky The ability of six-year-olds, eight-year-olds, and adults to abstract visual patterns. , 1974, Child development.

[39]  M. Posner,et al.  Retention of Abstract Ideas. , 1970 .

[40]  E. Rosch,et al.  Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories , 1975, Cognitive Psychology.

[41]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Categories and concepts , 1984 .