Left and right hand recognition in upper limb amputees.

Previous research suggests a close similarity in brain activity between mental simulation of a movement and its real counterpart. To explore this similarity, we aimed to assess whether imagery is affected by the loss of a limb or of its motor skills. We examined the performance of 16 adult, upper limb amputees (and age-matched controls) in a left/right hand judgement task that implicitly requires motor imagery. The experimental group included subjects who had suffered the amputation of the dominant or the non-dominant limb. Although responding well above chance, amputees as a group were slower and less accurate than controls. Nevertheless, their response pattern was similar to that of controls, namely slower response times and more errors for stimuli depicting hands in unnatural orientations, i.e. postures difficult to reach with a real movement. Interestingly, for all stimuli, amputees' performance was strongly affected by the side of limb loss: subjects who underwent amputation of their preferred limb made more errors and required greater latencies to respond as compared with amputees of the non-dominant limb. In a further analysis we observed that the habit of wearing an aesthetic prosthesis significantly interfered with the ability to judge the corresponding hand. Our data lead to three main conclusions: (i) loss of a single limb per se does not prevent motor imagery but it significantly enhances its difficulty; (ii) these subjects apparently perform the hand recognition task using a strategy in which they initially mentally simulate movements of their dominant limb; (iii) wearing a prosthesis, devoid of any motor function, seems to interfere with motor imagery, consistent with the view that only 'tools' can be incorporated in a dynamic body schema.

[1]  A. Sirigu,et al.  Laterality effects on motor awareness , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[2]  S. H. Johnson,et al.  Intact Motor Imagery in Chronic Upper Limb Hemiplegics: Evidence for Activity-Independent Action Representations , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[3]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Abnormalities in the awareness of action , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[4]  Tamar Flash,et al.  Computational approaches to motor control , 2001, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[5]  A. Sirigu,et al.  Motor and Visual Imagery as Two Complementary but Neurally Dissociable Mental Processes , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[6]  Atsushi Iriki,et al.  Self-images in the video monitor coded by monkey intraparietal neurons , 2001, Neuroscience Research.

[7]  J B Poline,et al.  Partially overlapping neural networks for real and imagined hand movements. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[8]  Peter Ford Dominey,et al.  Motor imagery in normal subjects and in asymmetrical Parkinson’s disease , 2000, Neurology.

[9]  Scott H. Johnson,et al.  Imagining the impossible: intact motor representations in hemiplegics , 2000, Neuroreport.

[10]  P. Maruff,et al.  Asymmetries between dominant and non-dominanthands in real and imagined motor task performance , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[11]  M S Gazzaniga,et al.  Cerebrally Lateralized Mental Representations of Hand Shape and Movement , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[12]  T. Takeda,et al.  Temporal structure of implicit motor imagery in visual hand‐shape discrimination as revealed by MEG , 1998, Neuroreport.

[13]  D. Crammond Motor imagery: never in your wildest dream , 1997, Trends in Neurosciences.

[14]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Localization of grasp representations in humans by positron emission tomography , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.

[15]  M. Tanaka,et al.  Coding of modified body schema during tool use by macaque postcentral neurones. , 1996, Neuroreport.

[16]  A. Sirigu,et al.  The Mental Representation of Hand Movements After Parietal Cortex Damage , 1996, Science.

[17]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Possible involvement of primary motor cortex in mentally simulated movement: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. , 1996, Neuroreport.

[18]  V. Ramachandran,et al.  Synaesthesia in phantom limbs induced with mirrors , 1996, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[19]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Mental motor imagery: a window into the representational stages of action , 1995, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[20]  M. Petrides,et al.  Neural correlates of mental transformations of the body-in-space. , 1995, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[21]  M. Jeannerod Mental imagery in the motor context , 1995, Neuropsychologia.

[22]  Peter Ford Dominey,et al.  Motor imagery of a lateralized sequential task is asymmetrically slowed in hemi-Parkinson's patients , 1995, Neuropsychologia.

[23]  Y Agid,et al.  Congruent unilateral impairments for real and imagined hand movements , 1995, Neuroreport.

[24]  L. Parsons,et al.  Use of implicit motor imagery for visual shape discrimination as revealed by PET , 1995, Nature.

[25]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Mapping motor representations with positron emission tomography , 1994, Nature.

[26]  L. Parsons Temporal and kinematic properties of motor behavior reflected in mentally simulated action. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[27]  J. Decety,et al.  Brain structures participating in mental simulation of motor behavior: a neuropsychological interpretation. , 1990, Acta psychologica.

[28]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  The timing of mentally represented actions , 1989, Behavioural Brain Research.

[29]  L M Parsons,et al.  Imagined spatial transformation of one's body. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[30]  L. Parsons Imagined spatial transformations of one's hands and feet , 1987, Cognitive Psychology.

[31]  K. Sekiyama,et al.  Kinesthetic aspects of mental representations in the identification of left and right hands , 1982, Perception & psychophysics.

[32]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[33]  M. Gentilucci,et al.  Right-handers and left-handers have different representations of their own hand. , 1998, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[34]  R. Passingham,et al.  Functional anatomy of the mental representation of upper extremity movements in healthy subjects. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.