Selective narrowing of social networks across adulthood is associated with improved emotional experience in daily life

Past research has documented age differences in the size and composition of social networks that suggest that networks grow smaller with age and include an increasingly greater proportion of well-known social partners. According to socioemotional selectivity theory, such changes in social network composition serve an antecedent emotion regulatory function that supports an age-related increase in the priority that people place on emotional well-being. The present study employed a longitudinal design with a sample that spanned the full adult age range to examine whether there is evidence of within-individual (developmental) change in social networks and whether the characteristics of relationships predict emotional experiences in daily life. Using growth curve analyses, social networks were found to increase in size in young adulthood and then decline steadily throughout later life. As postulated by socioemotional selectivity theory, reductions were observed primarily in the number of peripheral partners; the number of close partners was relatively stable over time. In addition, cross-sectional analyses revealed that older adults reported that social network members elicited less negative emotion and more positive emotion. The emotional tone of social networks, particularly when negative emotions were associated with network members, predicted daily emotional experience. These findings were robust after taking into account demographic variables and physical health. The implications of these findings are discussed in the context of socioemotional selectivity theory and related theoretical models.

[1]  D. Mroczek,et al.  The effect of age on positive and negative affect: a developmental perspective on happiness. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[2]  L. Carstensen,et al.  Emotion and aging: experience, expression, and control. , 1997, Psychology and aging.

[3]  Paul B. Baltes,et al.  The Berlin Aging Study , 1994, Ageing and Society.

[4]  J. Gross The Emerging Field of Emotion Regulation: An Integrative Review , 1998 .

[5]  L. Carstensen,et al.  The Process of Successful Ageing , 1996, Ageing and Society.

[6]  R Kliegl,et al.  Speed and intelligence in old age. , 1993, Psychology and aging.

[7]  L. Carstensen,et al.  Emotional experience improves with age: evidence based on over 10 years of experience sampling. , 2011, Psychology and aging.

[8]  F. Blanchard-Fields,et al.  Age differences in emotion-regulation strategies in handling everyday problems. , 2004, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[9]  B. Ingersoll-Dayton,et al.  The effects of positive and negative social exchanges on aging adults. , 1997, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[10]  L. Carstensen,et al.  Emotional experience in everyday life across the adult life span. , 2000, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[11]  A. Stone,et al.  A snapshot of the age distribution of psychological well-being in the United States , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[12]  L. Carstensen,et al.  The Social Context of Emotional Experience , 1997, Annual Review of Gerontology and Geriatrics.

[13]  L. Carstensen The Influence of a Sense of Time on Human Development , 2006, Science.

[14]  C. Wrzus,et al.  Social network changes and life events across the life span: a meta-analysis. , 2013, Psychological bulletin.

[15]  U. Lindenberger,et al.  Seeking Pleasure and Seeking Pain: Differences in Prohedonic and Contra-Hedonic Motivation From Adolescence to Old Age , 2009, Psychological science.

[16]  C. Reynolds,et al.  Age-related differences and change in positive and negative affect over 23 years. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[17]  Frieder R Lang,et al.  Blood Is Thicker Than Water : Kinship Orientation Across Adulthood , 2003 .

[18]  Robert L. Kahn,et al.  Convoys over the life course: attachment, roles, and social support , 1980 .

[19]  J. Nesselroade The warp and the woof of the developmental fabric. , 1991 .

[20]  Susanne Scheibe,et al.  Effects of regulating emotions on cognitive performance: what is costly for young adults is not so costly for older adults. , 2009, Psychology and aging.

[21]  Susanne Scheibe,et al.  Emotional aging: recent findings and future trends. , 2010, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[22]  U. Staudinger,et al.  Affective experience in adulthood and old age: The role of affective arousal and perceived affect regulation. , 2009, Psychology and aging.

[23]  M. Mather,et al.  Aging and motivated cognition: the positivity effect in attention and memory , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[24]  Roberto Cabeza,et al.  The Cambridge Handbook of Human Affective Neuroscience: Emotion and Aging , 2013 .

[25]  R. Levenson,et al.  Effects of Aging on Experimentally Instructed Detached Reappraisal, Positive Reappraisal, and Emotional Behavior Suppression Aging and Emotion Regulation: the Current Picture , 2022 .

[26]  P. Baltes,et al.  Psychological perspectives on successful aging: The model of selective optimization with compensation. , 1990 .

[27]  Lisa Emery,et al.  Cognitive consequences of expressive regulation in older adults. , 2011, Psychology and aging.

[28]  L. Carstensen Social and emotional patterns in adulthood: support for socioemotional selectivity theory. , 1992, Psychology and aging.

[29]  T. Dening The Berlin Aging Study: Aging from 70 to 100. Edited by P. B. Baltes and K. U. Mayer. (Pp. 552: £45.00.) Cambridge University Press: Cambridge. 1999. , 2000, Psychological Medicine.

[30]  S. Charles,et al.  Better with age: Social relationships across adulthood , 2011, Journal of social and personal relationships.

[31]  L. Carstensen,et al.  Taking time seriously. A theory of socioemotional selectivity. , 1999, The American psychologist.

[32]  Franz J. Neyer,et al.  Personal effort in social relationships across adulthood. , 2013, Psychology and aging.

[33]  M. Lawton,et al.  Dimensions of affective experience in three age groups. , 1992, Psychology and aging.

[34]  K. Rook,et al.  The negative side of social interaction: impact on psychological well-being. , 1984, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[35]  Frieder R. Lang,et al.  Endings and Continuity of Social Relationships: Maximizing Intrinsic Benefits within Personal Networks when Feeling Near to Death , 2000 .

[36]  Shu-Chen Li,et al.  Intraindividual variability in positive and negative affect over 45 days: do older adults fluctuate less than young adults? , 2009, Psychology and aging.

[37]  K. Fingerman,et al.  Do we get better at picking our battles? Age group differences in descriptions of behavioral reactions to interpersonal tensions. , 2005, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[38]  Laura L. Carstensen,et al.  At the Intersection of Emotion and Cognition , 2005 .