Fifth-grade students’ approaches to and beliefs of mathematics word problem solving: a large sample Hungarian study

Mathematical word problems used in Verschaffel et al.’s (Learning and Instruction 7:339–359, 1994) study were applied in several follow-up studies. The goal of the present study was to replicate and extend the results of this line of research in a large sample of Hungarian students using an alternative set of data-gathering and data-analysis techniques. 4,037 students forming a nationwide representative sample of the Hungarian fifth-grade student population (aged 10–11) completed the test. The test contained five word problems from the list of 10 P(‘problematic)-items from Verschaffel et al.’s test. In contrast to all previous research in this domain, we used a multiple-choice format, where three options were given for each task: (a) routine-based, non-realistic answer, (b) numerical response that does take into account realistic considerations, (c) a realistic solution stating that the task cannot be solved. The hypotheses of this study were: (1) Students’ responses will confirm previous results, i.e. upper elementary school students prefer to respond to P-items by means of the routine-based answer; (2) Most students will demonstrate a more or less consistent preference for a given answer type (a, b or c) over problems; (3) Students’ school math marks will have low correlation indices with students’ achievement on these word problems. Our results confirm student’s overall tendency to follow non-realistic approaches when doing school word problem solving. The tendency even holds when confronting students with various kinds of realistic answers. Our results show that students demonstrate response patterns over problems, and that the correlation with math school performance is significant but small.

[1]  H. O. Pollak,et al.  How can we teach applications of mathematics? , 1969 .

[2]  John A. Sloboda,et al.  Cognitive processes in mathematics , 1987 .

[3]  Magdalene Lampert,et al.  When the Problem Is Not the Question and the Solution Is Not the Answer: Mathematical Knowing and Teaching , 1990 .

[4]  D. Perkins,et al.  Informal reasoning and education , 1991 .

[5]  A. Schoenfeld On mathematics as sense-making: An informal attack on the unfortunate divorce of formal and informal mathematics. , 1991 .

[6]  A. Schoenfeld Learning to Think Mathematically: Problem Solving, Metacognition, and Sense Making in Mathematics (Reprint) , 2009 .

[7]  B. Greer Multiplication and division as models of situations. , 1992 .

[8]  Brian Greer,et al.  The Mathematical Modeling Perspective on Wor(l)d Problems. , 1993 .

[9]  Lieven Verschaffel,et al.  A decade of research on word problem solving in Leuven: Theoretical, methodological, and practical outcomes , 1993 .

[10]  E. Corte,et al.  Realistic considerations in mathematical modeling of school arithmetic word problems , 1994 .

[11]  Mary Hegarty,et al.  The process of understanding mathematical problems. , 1996 .

[12]  J. -. Czyba,et al.  L'âge du capitaine , 1996 .

[13]  P. Cobb,et al.  Sociomathematical Norms, Argumentation, and Autonomy in Mathematics. , 1996 .

[14]  Robert J. Sternberg,et al.  The Nature of Mathematical Thinking , 1996 .

[15]  Jan Wyndhamn,et al.  Word problems and mathematical reasoning—A study of children's mastery of reference and meaning in textual realities , 1997 .

[16]  Kurt Reusser,et al.  Every word problem has a solution—The social rationality of mathematical modeling in schools , 1997 .

[17]  Lieven Verschaffel,et al.  Pre-service teachers' conceptions and beliefs about the role of real-world knowledge in mathematical modelling of school word problems , 1997 .

[18]  B. Greer Modelling Reality in Mathematics Classrooms: The Case of Word Problems. , 1997 .

[19]  Terezinha Nunes,et al.  Learning and teaching mathematics : an international perspective , 1997 .

[20]  Lieven Verschaffel,et al.  Word problems: A vehicle for promoting authentic mathematical understanding and problem solving in the primary school? , 1997 .

[21]  Barry Cooper,et al.  Anyone for Tennis? Social Class Differences in Children's Responses to National Curriculum Mathematics Testing , 1998 .

[22]  E. Corte,et al.  Making sense of word problems , 2000 .

[23]  Barry Cooper,et al.  Children's Responses to Contrasting `Realistic' Mathematics Problems: Just how realistic are children ready to be? , 2002 .

[24]  E. Corte,et al.  Students' Mathematics-Related Belief Systems: Design and Analysis of a Questionnaire. , 2003 .

[25]  E. Pehkonen,et al.  On relationships between beliefs and knowledge in mathematics education , 2003 .

[26]  Arithmetic word-problem-solving in Huntington’s disease , 2005, Brain and Cognition.

[27]  Mary Jane White,et al.  Implicit writing beliefs and their relation to writing quality , 2005 .

[28]  Ernest C. D. M. van Lieshout,et al.  The effect of illustrations in arithmetic problem-solving: Effects of increased cognitive load , 2009 .

[29]  Rainer Bromme,et al.  Who knows what and who can we believe? Epistemological beliefs are beliefs about knowledge (mostly) to be attained from others , 2010 .

[30]  Lisa D. Bendixen,et al.  Personal Epistemology in the Classroom: Conclusion , 2010 .

[31]  Lieven Verschaffel,et al.  Reconceptualising Word Problems as Exercises in Mathematical Modelling , 2010 .