Declarative Memory, Awareness, and Transitive Inference

A characteristic usually attributed to declarative memory is that what is learned is accessible to awareness. Recently, the relationship between awareness and declarative (hippocampus-dependent) memory has been questioned on the basis of findings from transitive inference tasks. In transitive inference, participants are first trained on overlapping pairs of items (e.g., A+B-, B+C-, C+D-, and D+E-, where + and - indicate correct and incorrect choices). Later, participants who choose B over D when presented with the novel pair BD are said to demonstrate transitive inference. The ability to exhibit transitive inference is thought to depend on the fact that participants have represented the stimulus elements hierarchically (i.e., A>B>C>D>E). We found that performance on five-item and six-item transitive inference tasks was closely related to awareness of the hierarchical relationship among the elements of the training pairs. Participants who were aware of the hierarchy performed near 100% correct on all tests of transitivity, but participants who were unaware of the hierarchy performed poorly (e.g., on transitive pair BD in the five-item problem; on transitive pairs BD, BE, and CE in the six-item problem). When the five-item task was administered to memory-impaired patients with damage thought to be limited to the hippocampal region, the patients were impaired at learning the training pairs. All patients were unaware of the hierarchy and, like unaware controls, performed poorly on the BD pair. The findings indicate that awareness is critical for robust performance on tests of transitive inference and support the view that awareness of what is learned is a fundamental characteristic of declarative memory.

[1]  G. Paxinos,et al.  THE HUMAN NERVOUS SYSTEM , 1975 .

[2]  Ramona O. Hopkins,et al.  The Neuroanatomy of Remote Memory , 2005, Neuron.

[3]  H. Soininen,et al.  MR volumetric analysis of the human entorhinal, perirhinal, and temporopolar cortices. , 1998, AJNR. American journal of neuroradiology.

[4]  Juan D Delius,et al.  Transitive responding in animals and humans: Exaptation rather than adaptation? , 1998, Behavioural Processes.

[5]  J. Gabrieli Cognitive neuroscience of human memory. , 1998, Annual review of psychology.

[6]  L. Squire,et al.  The Neuropsychology of Memory , 1990 .

[7]  D. Amaral,et al.  Entorhinal Cortex Lesions Disrupt the Relational Organization of Memory in Monkeys , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[8]  Transitive inference and awareness in humans , 2004, Behavioural Processes.

[9]  M. Siemann,et al.  Influences of task concreteness upon transitive responding in humans , 1996, Psychological research.

[10]  M. Mishkin,et al.  Visual habit formation in monkeys with neurotoxic lesions of the ventrocaudal neostriatum , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[11]  Larry R Squire,et al.  Quantifying medial temporal lobe damage in memory‐impaired patients † , 2005, Hippocampus.

[12]  Suzanne Corkin,et al.  Semantic knowledge in patient H.M. and other patients with bilateral medial and lateral temporal lobe lesions , 2002, Hippocampus.

[13]  L. Squire,et al.  Robust habit learning in the absence of awareness and independent of the medial temporal lobe , 2005, Nature.

[14]  Gurindar S. Sohi,et al.  Memory systems , 1996, CSUR.

[15]  D. Levy,et al.  Transitive inference in schizophrenia: impairments in relational memory organization , 2004, Schizophrenia Research.

[16]  L. Squire,et al.  Contrasting Effects on Discrimination Learning after Hippocampal Lesions and Conjoint Hippocampal–Caudate Lesions in Monkeys , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[17]  L. Squire,et al.  A reexamination of the concurrent discrimination learning task , 1998 .

[18]  H. Eichenbaum,et al.  The hippocampus and memory for orderly stimulus relations. , 1997, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[19]  Thomas R. Zentall,et al.  Transitive inference in pigeons , 1991 .

[20]  L R Squire,et al.  A reexamination of the concurrent discrimination learning task: the importance of anterior inferotemporal cortex, area TE. , 1998, Behavioral neuroscience.

[21]  H Eichenbaum,et al.  Declarative memory: insights from cognitive neurobiology. , 1997, Annual review of psychology.

[22]  Michael Van Elzakker,et al.  Transitivity, flexibility, conjunctive representations, and the hippocampus. I. An empirical analysis , 2003, Hippocampus.

[23]  E. Tulving,et al.  Memory Systems 1994 , 1994 .

[24]  H Eichenbaum,et al.  Learning and memory for hierarchical relationships in the monkey: effects of aging. , 1996, Behavioral neuroscience.

[25]  J. Delius,et al.  Implicit deductive responding in humans , 1993, Naturwissenschaften.

[26]  H. E. Rosvold,et al.  Behavioral effects of selective ablation of the caudate nucleus. , 1967, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[27]  H. Eichenbaum,et al.  From Conditioning to Conscious Recollection , 2001 .

[28]  L. Squire Memory and the hippocampus: a synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans. , 1992, Psychological review.

[29]  William B Levy,et al.  Relational learning with and without awareness: Transitive inference using nonverbal stimuli in humans , 2001, Memory & cognition.

[30]  William B. Levy,et al.  When logic fails: Implicit transitive inference in humans , 2005, Memory & cognition.

[31]  Characterizing amnesic patients for neurobehavioral study. , 1986 .

[32]  P. Osterrieth Le test de copie d'une figure complexe , 1944 .