Upper nasal hemifield location and nonspatial auditory tones accelerate visual detection during dichoptic viewing
暂无分享,去创建一个
Jukka Häkkinen | Terhi Mustonen | Lari Vainio | Mikko Nuutinen | L. Vainio | J. Häkkinen | T. Mustonen | M. Nuutinen
[1] N. Logothetis,et al. Visual competition , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[2] P. Sterzer,et al. Unconscious processing under interocular suppression: getting the right measure , 2014, Front. Psychol..
[3] C. Spence. Crossmodal correspondences: A tutorial review , 2011, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[4] J. Theeuwes,et al. Attention and the multiple stages of multisensory integration: A review of audiovisual studies. , 2010, Acta psychologica.
[5] 日本規格協会. Acoustics : normal equal-loudness-level contours = 音響 : 正常な音の大きさの等感曲線 , 2004 .
[6] Aaron R. Seitz,et al. Benefits of multisensory learning , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[7] Anne Treisman,et al. Natural cross-modal mappings between visual and auditory features. , 2011, Journal of vision.
[8] Michael W. Levine,et al. The relative capabilities of the upper and lower visual hemifields , 2005, Vision Research.
[9] Chris L. E. Paffen,et al. Breaking continuous flash suppression: competing for consciousness on the pre-semantic battlefield , 2014, Front. Psychol..
[10] Miguel Castelo-Branco,et al. Asymmetry of visual sensory mechanisms: electrophysiological, structural, and psychophysical evidences. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[11] R. Blake,et al. A new interocular suppression technique for measuring sensory eye dominance. , 2010, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.
[12] Aaron R. Seitz,et al. Sound Facilitates Visual Learning , 2006, Current Biology.
[13] A. Sack,et al. The hybrid model of attentional control: New insights into hemispheric asymmetries inferred from TMS research , 2015, Neuropsychologia.
[14] C. Westbury. Implicit sound symbolism in lexical access: Evidence from an interference task , 2005, Brain and Language.
[15] J. Sergent. The cerebral balance of power: confrontation or cooperation? , 1982, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[16] P. Cavanagh,et al. Attentional resolution and the locus of visual awareness , 1996, Nature.
[17] Louise Kauffmann,et al. The neural bases of spatial frequency processing during scene perception , 2014, Front. Integr. Neurosci..
[18] Geraint Rees,et al. Visual FMRI responses in human superior colliculus show a temporal-nasal asymmetry that is absent in lateral geniculate and visual cortex. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.
[19] Randolph Blake,et al. Depth of interocular suppression associated with continuous flash suppression, flash suppression, and binocular rivalry. , 2006, Journal of vision.
[20] C. Curcio,et al. Topography of ganglion cells in human retina , 1990, The Journal of comparative neurology.
[21] P. Bertelson,et al. The time-course of preparation: Confirmatory results with visual and auditory warning signals , 1969 .
[22] M. Fahle,et al. Naso-temporal asymmetry of visual perception and of the visual cortex , 1988, Vision Research.
[23] Su-Ling Yeh,et al. Accessing the meaning of invisible words , 2011, Consciousness and Cognition.
[24] A. Henik,et al. The Endogenous Modulation of IOR is Nasal-Temporal Asymmetric , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[25] Philipp Sterzer,et al. Neural processing of visual information under interocular suppression: a critical review , 2014, Front. Psychol..
[26] S. Los,et al. Intentional and unintentional contributions to nonspecific preparation during reaction time foreperiods. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[27] R. van Ee,et al. Suppressed visual looming stimuli are not integrated with auditory looming signals: Evidence from continuous flash suppression , 2015, i-Perception.
[28] Miguel Castelo-Branco,et al. Retinal and cortical patterns of spatial anisotropy in contrast sensitivity tasks , 2008, Vision Research.
[29] Jukka Häkkinen,et al. CFS MATLAB toolbox: An experiment builder for continuous flash suppression (CFS) task , 2017, Behavior Research Methods.
[30] M. Kleiner,et al. Audiovisual interactions in binocular rivalry. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[31] K. Rayner. Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. , 1998, Psychological bulletin.
[32] Robert S. Laramee,et al. Rivalry and interference with a head-mounted display , 2002, TCHI.
[33] R. C. Oldfield. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.
[34] S. Christman,et al. Upper and lower visual field differences in categorical and coordinate judgments , 1998 .
[35] Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al. The Scope and Limits of Top-Down Attention in Unconscious Visual Processing , 2006, Current Biology.
[36] Denis G. Pelli,et al. ECVP '07 Abstracts , 2007, Perception.
[37] Robert Fox,et al. Binocular Rivalry and Head-Worn Displays , 2007, Hum. Factors.
[38] Charles Spence,et al. Audiovisual crossmodal correspondences and sound symbolism: a study using the implicit association test , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.
[39] Marcia Grabowecky,et al. Changes in auditory frequency guide visual–spatial attention , 2011, Cognition.
[40] Anina N. Rich,et al. Cross-Modality Correspondence between Pitch and Spatial Location Modulates Attentional Orienting , 2012, Perception.
[41] David Mumford,et al. Occlusion Models for Natural Images: A Statistical Study of a Scale-Invariant Dead Leaves Model , 2004, International Journal of Computer Vision.
[42] Kevin G. Munhall,et al. Detection of Audiovisual Speech Correspondences Without Visual Awareness , 2013, Psychological science.
[43] P. Kaushall. FUNCTIONAL ASYMMETRIES OF THE HUMAN VISUAL SYSTEM AS REVEALED BY BINOCULAR RIVALRY AND BINOCULAR BRIGHTNESS MATCHING* , 1975, American journal of optometry and physiological optics.
[44] Takashi Kawai,et al. Visual task performance using a monocular see-through head-mounted display (HMD) while walking. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.
[45] Randolph Blake,et al. Deconstructing continuous flash suppression. , 2012, Journal of vision.
[46] P. Hsieh,et al. Can a Word Sound Like a Shape Before You Have Seen It? Sound-Shape Mapping Prior to Conscious Awareness , 2017, Psychological science.
[47] M. Morrone,et al. Auditory and Tactile Signals Combine to Influence Vision during Binocular Rivalry , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[48] Sheng He,et al. Temporal characteristics of binocular rivalry: visual field asymmetries , 2003, Vision Research.
[49] C. Koch,et al. Continuous flash suppression reduces negative afterimages , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.
[50] Su-Ling Yeh,et al. Unmasking the dichoptic mask by sound: spatial congruency matters , 2014, Experimental Brain Research.
[51] Sheng He,et al. Processing of Invisible Stimuli: Advantage of Upright Faces and Recognizable Words in Overcoming Interocular Suppression , 2007, Psychological science.
[52] Jan Theeuwes,et al. Pip and pop: nonspatial auditory signals improve spatial visual search. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[53] I. Spence,et al. Upper visual field advantage in localizing a target among distractors , 2014, i-Perception.
[54] M. Niemeier,et al. Paradoxical cross-over due to attention to high or low spatial frequencies , 2008, Brain and Cognition.
[55] P. Reuter-Lorenz,et al. Vertical orienting control: evidence for attentional bias and "neglect" in the intact brain. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[56] D. V. van Essen,et al. The representation of the visual field in parvicellular and magnocellular layers of the lateral geniculate nucleus in the macaque monkey , 1984, The Journal of comparative neurology.
[57] P. Cavanagh,et al. The Spatial Resolution of Visual Attention , 2001, Cognitive Psychology.
[58] George M. Gould,et al. A METHOD OF DETERMINING OCULAR DOMINANCE , 1910 .
[59] A. Hendrickson,et al. Human photoreceptor topography , 1990, The Journal of comparative neurology.
[60] Hiroshi Uozato,et al. Effects of Dominant and Nondominant Eyes in Binocular Rivalry , 2004, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.
[61] M. Carrasco,et al. Characterizing visual performance fields: effects of transient covert attention, spatial frequency, eccentricity, task and set size. , 2001, Spatial vision.
[62] J. Rovamo,et al. Visual resolution, contrast sensitivity, and the cortical magnification factor , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[63] Wolfgang Sanz,et al. Laser-optical investigation of stator-rotor interaction in a transonic turbine , 2006, J. Vis..
[64] Randolph Blake,et al. Stimulus Fractionation by Interocular Suppression , 2011, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[65] Su-Ling Yeh,et al. Crossmodal Constraints on Human Perceptual Awareness: Auditory Semantic Modulation of Binocular Rivalry , 2011, Front. Psychology.
[66] G. Yovel,et al. Hemispheric asymmetries for global and local visual perception: effects of stimulus and task factors. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[67] F. Previc. Functional specialization in the lower and upper visual fields in humans: Its ecological origins and neurophysiological implications , 1990, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[68] M. McCourt,et al. Pseudoneglect: a review and meta-analysis of performance factors in line bisection tasks , 2000, Neuropsychologia.
[69] Victoria J. Bourne. The divided visual field paradigm: Methodological considerations , 2006, Laterality.