Absence of distracting information explains the redundant signals effect for a centrally presented categorization task.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] M. Tamietto,et al. Fast recognition of social emotions takes the whole brain: Interhemispheric cooperation in the absence of cerebral asymmetry , 2007, Neuropsychologia.
[2] Michael C Corballis,et al. Hemispheric interactions in simple reaction time , 2002, Neuropsychologia.
[3] Stefan R Schweinberger,et al. Interhemispheric cooperation for familiar but not unfamiliar face processing , 2002, Neuropsychologia.
[4] J. Hellige,et al. Interhemispheric interaction in bilateral redundancy gain: effects of stimulus format. , 2003, Neuropsychology.
[5] Jeff Miller,et al. Effects of redundant visual stimuli on temporal order judgments , 2004, Perception & psychophysics.
[6] Richard D. Morey,et al. Confidence Intervals from Normalized Data: A correction to Cousineau (2005) , 2008 .
[7] Elizabeth L Bjork,et al. On the nature of input channels in visual processing. , 1977, Psychological review.
[8] C. Marzi,et al. Does the redundant signal effect occur at an early visual stage? , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.
[9] J. Hellige,et al. Benefits of interhemispheric collaboration can be eliminated by mixing stimulus formats that involve different cortical access routes , 2007, Brain and Cognition.
[10] Joseph Krummenacher,et al. Visual search for dimensionally redundant pop-out targets: parallel-coactive processing of dimensions is location specific. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[11] David A. Taylor. Identification and Categorization of Letters and Digits. , 1978 .
[12] M. Tamietto,et al. Functional asymmetry and interhemispheric cooperation in the perception of emotions from facial expressions , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.
[13] S Yantis,et al. Dividing attention between color and shape: Evidence of coactivation , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.
[14] A Pfefferbaum,et al. Parallel interhemispheric processing in aging and alcoholism: relation to corpus callosum size , 2004, Neuropsychologia.
[15] Margot J. Taylor,et al. Accelerated and amplified neural responses in visual discrimination: Two features are processed faster than one , 2005, NeuroImage.
[16] J. T. Mordkoff,et al. Dividing attention between color and shape revisited: redundant targets coactivate only when parts of the same perceptual object , 2011, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[17] E Bricolo,et al. Redundancy gain in the stop-signal paradigm: implications for the locus of coactivation in simple reaction time. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[18] M Giray,et al. Motor coactivation revealed by response force in divided and focused attention. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[19] S. B. Vincent. The function of the vibrissae in the behavior of the white rat , 1912 .
[20] Jeff Miller,et al. Systematic biases and Type I error accumulation in tests of the race model inequality , 2007, Behavior research methods.
[21] Stephen K. Reed,et al. Psychological processes in pattern recognition , 1973 .
[22] W Schwarz,et al. Coactivation and Statistical Facilitation in the Detection of Lines , 1994, Perception.
[23] T. Wüstenberg,et al. Parallel interhemispheric processing in hemineglect: Relation to visual field defects , 2009, Neuropsychologia.
[24] M. Girelli,et al. Neural Site of the Redundant Target Effect: Electrophysiological Evidence , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[25] Edgar Erdfelder,et al. G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences , 2007, Behavior research methods.
[26] F. Lepore,et al. Impact of the spatial congruence of redundant targets on within-modal and cross-modal integration , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.
[27] Michael C Corballis,et al. Interhemispheric neural summation in the split brain with symmetrical and asymmetrical displays , 2002, Neuropsychologia.
[28] Stefan Pollmann,et al. Neural basis of redundancy effects in visual object categorization , 2007, Neuroscience Letters.
[29] G R Grice,et al. Combination rule for redundant information in reaction time tasks with divided attention , 1984, Perception & psychophysics.
[30] C. Eriksen,et al. Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .
[31] S. Sternberg. Memory-scanning: mental processes revealed by reaction-time experiments. , 1969, American scientist.
[32] Jeff Miller,et al. Timecourse of coactivation in bimodal divided attention , 1986, Perception & psychophysics.
[33] D. Raab. DIVISION OF PSYCHOLOGY: STATISTICAL FACILITATION OF SIMPLE REACTION TIMES* , 1962 .
[34] Marco Iacoboni,et al. Interhemispheric visuo‐motor integration in humans: the effect of redundant targets , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.
[35] Elkan G. Akyürek,et al. The electrophysiological locus of the redundant target effect on visual discrimination in a dual singleton search task , 2013, Brain Research.
[36] M. Corballis,et al. Decisions about identity and orientation of rotated letters and digits , 1978, Memory & cognition.
[37] G R Grice,et al. Absence of a redundant-signals effect in a reaction time task with divided attention , 1984, Perception & psychophysics.
[38] D. Madden,et al. Influence of response selection and noise similarity on age differences in the redundancy gain. , 1993, Journal of gerontology.
[39] Jeff Miller,et al. Divided attention: Evidence for coactivation with redundant signals , 1982, Cognitive Psychology.
[40] J. Jonides,et al. A conceptual category effect in visual search: O as letter or as digit , 1972 .
[41] G R Grice,et al. Redundancy phenomena are affected by response requirements , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.
[42] A. Diederich. Intersensory facilitation of reaction time: evaluation of counter and diffusion coactivation models , 1995 .
[43] D J Madden,et al. Adult age differences in attention: filtering or selection? , 1994, Journal of gerontology.
[44] Jeff Miller,et al. Exaggerated redundancy gain in the split brain: A hemispheric coactivation account , 2004, Cognitive Psychology.
[45] Cristiana Cavina-Pratesi,et al. Redundant target effect and intersensory facilitation from visual-tactile interactions in simple reaction time , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.
[46] Mark B. Neider,et al. Display Configuration and Duration Effects in Redundancy Gain for a Categorization Task , 2017 .
[47] G. Grice,et al. Dependence of target redundancy effects on noise conditions and number of targets , 1987, Perception & psychophysics.
[48] M. Posner. On the Relationship between Letter Names and Superordinate Categories , 1970 .
[49] P. Praamstra,et al. Redundant-signals effects on reaction time, response force, and movement-related potentials in Parkinson's disease , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.
[50] D. Madden,et al. Age-related differences in the processing of redundant visual dimensions. , 2005, Psychology and aging.
[51] Denis Cousineau,et al. Confidence intervals in within-subject designs: A simpler solution to Loftus and Masson's method , 2005 .
[52] Michael C Corballis,et al. Interhemispheric neural summation in the split brain: effects of stimulus colour and task , 2003, Neuropsychologia.
[53] Hans Colonius,et al. Intersensory facilitation in the motor component? , 1987 .
[54] H. Müller,et al. The detection of feature singletons defined in two dimensions is based on salience summation, rather than on serial exhaustive or interactive race architectures , 2009, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[55] G R Grice,et al. What makes targets redundant? , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.
[56] A. O. Dick. Processing time for naming and categorization of letters and numbers , 1971 .
[57] Paul M. Corballis,et al. Paradoxical Interhemispheric Summation in the Split Brain , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[58] C. Pernet,et al. Neural timing of visual implicit categorization. , 2003, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[59] Jeff Miller,et al. Sensory and motor involvement in the enhanced redundant target effect: A study comparing anterior- and totally split-brain individuals , 2009, Neuropsychologia.
[60] Stefan R Schweinberger,et al. Interhemispheric cooperation for face recognition but not for affective facial expressions , 2003, Neuropsychologia.
[61] M. White. Naming and categorization of tilted alphanumeric characters do not require mental rotation , 1980 .
[62] D. Raab. Statistical facilitation of simple reaction times. , 1962, Transactions of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[63] W. Schwarz. On the relationship between the redundant signals effect and temporal order judgments: parametric data and a new model. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[64] M. Carrasco,et al. Isoeccentric locations are not equivalent: The extent of the vertical meridian asymmetry , 2012, Vision Research.
[65] Rolf Ulrich,et al. Testing the race model inequality: An algorithm and computer programs , 2007, Behavior research methods.
[66] Jeff Miller,et al. Locus of the redundant-signals effect in bimodal divided attention: A neurophysiological analysis , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.
[67] J Miller,et al. Absence of coactivation in the motor component: evidence from psychophysiological measures of target detection. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[68] Uri Feintuch,et al. Visual attention and coactivation of response decisions for features from different dimensions. , 2002, Psychological science.