The neural basis of tool use.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] E. Renzi,et al. Ideational apraxia: A quantitative study , 1968 .
[2] H. Steinthal. Abriss der Sprachwissenschaft , 1972 .
[3] J. Gibson. The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1979 .
[4] [The syndrome of ideational apraxia and its localisation]. , 1980, Der Nervenarzt.
[5] T. Shallice. Specific impairments of planning. , 1982, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.
[6] Colin Potts,et al. Design of Everyday Things , 1988 .
[7] Richard J. Brown. Neuropsychology Mental Structure , 1989 .
[8] M. Torrens. Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain—3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging, J. Talairach, P. Tournoux. Georg Thieme Verlag, New York (1988), 122 pp., 130 figs. DM 268 , 1990 .
[9] John Hart,et al. Delineation of single‐word semantic comprehension deficits in aphasia, with anatomical correlation , 1990, Annals of neurology.
[10] M Poncet,et al. The role of sensorimotor experience in object recognition. A case of multimodal agnosia. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[11] Marie-Christine Jaulent,et al. Object structure and action requirements: A compatibility model for functional recognition , 1991, Int. J. Intell. Syst..
[12] K M Heilman,et al. Conceptual apraxia in Alzheimer's disease. , 1992, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[13] J. Fuster. Frontal lobes , 1993, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[14] N. Motomura,et al. A Case of Ideational Apraxia with Impairment of Object Use and Preservation of Object Pantomime , 1994, Cortex.
[15] G. Gainotti,et al. Neuroanatomical correlates of category-specific semantic disorders: a critical survey. , 1995, Memory.
[16] Angela Sirigu,et al. A Selective Impairment of Hand Posture for Object Utilization in Apraxia , 1995, Cortex.
[17] K M Heilman,et al. Ecological implications of limb apraxia: Evidence from mealtime behavior , 1995, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.
[18] A. Damasio,et al. A neural basis for lexical retrieval , 1996, Nature.
[19] R. Cabeza,et al. Imaging Cognition: An Empirical Review of PET Studies with Normal Subjects , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[20] K M Heilman,et al. Conceptual apraxia from lateralized lesions , 1997, Neurology.
[21] Michael W. Montgomery,et al. Naturalistic action impairment in closed head injury. , 1998, Neuropsychology.
[22] G. Goldenberg,et al. Tool use and mechanical problem solving in apraxia , 1998, Neuropsychologia.
[23] G. Humphreys,et al. Disordered action schema and action disorganisation syndrome , 1998 .
[24] Laurel J Buxbaum,et al. Naturalistic action production following right hemisphere stroke , 1998, Neuropsychologia.
[25] J R Hodges,et al. "What" and "how": evidence for the dissociation of object knowledge and mechanical problem-solving skills in the human brain. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[26] J. Hermsdörfer,et al. Effects of unilateral brain damage on grip selection, coordination, and kinematics of ipsilesional prehension , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.
[27] A. Sunderland,et al. Impaired dexterity of the ipsilateral hand after stroke and the relationship to cognitive deficit. , 1999, Stroke.
[28] M. L. Lambon Ralph,et al. The role of conceptual knowledge in object use evidence from semantic dementia. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[29] M P Young,et al. On imputing function to structure from the behavioural effects of brain lesions. , 2000, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.
[30] C. Rorden,et al. Stereotaxic display of brain lesions. , 2000, Behavioural neurology.
[31] Using a fixed provisional as an orthodontic anchor in forced eruption. , 2000, Practical periodontics and aesthetic dentistry : PPAD.
[32] Alex Martin,et al. Representation of Manipulable Man-Made Objects in the Dorsal Stream , 2000, NeuroImage.
[33] H. Freund,et al. Recognition and imitation of pantomimed motor acts after unilateral parietal and premotor lesions: a perspective on apraxia , 2001, Neuropsychologia.
[34] T Shallice,et al. A Form of Ideational Apraxia as a Delective Deficit of Contention Scheduling , 2001, Cognitive neuropsychology.
[35] P. Strick,et al. Imaging the premotor areas , 2001, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[36] L. Buxbaum. Ideomotor Apraxia: a Call to Action , 2001, Neurocase.
[37] N. Tzourio-Mazoyer,et al. Automated Anatomical Labeling of Activations in SPM Using a Macroscopic Anatomical Parcellation of the MNI MRI Single-Subject Brain , 2002, NeuroImage.
[38] T. Fukutake,et al. Apraxia of Tool Use: An Autopsy Case of Biparietal Infarction , 2002, European Neurology.
[39] J. Decety,et al. Does visual perception of object afford action? Evidence from a neuroimaging study , 2002, Neuropsychologia.
[40] Karalyn Patterson,et al. Apraxia, mechanical problem solving and semantic knowledge: contributions to object usage in corticobasal degeneration. , 2002, Journal of neurology.
[41] J. Hodges,et al. When objects lose their meaning: What happens to their use? , 2002, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.
[42] Thomas E. Nichols,et al. Thresholding of Statistical Maps in Functional Neuroimaging Using the False Discovery Rate , 2002, NeuroImage.
[43] R. Adolphs,et al. NEURAL CORRELATES OF CONCEPTUAL KNOWLEDGE FOR ACTIONS , 2003, Cognitive neuropsychology.
[44] M. Brett,et al. Actions Speak Louder Than Functions: The Importance of Manipulability and Action in Tool Representation , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[45] D. Povinelli. Folk physics for apes : the chimpanzee's theory of how the world works , 2003 .
[46] M. Hegarty. Mechanical reasoning by mental simulation , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[47] H. Karnath,et al. Using human brain lesions to infer function: a relic from a past era in the fMRI age? , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[48] K. Poeck. Ideational apraxia , 2004, Journal of Neurology.
[49] Sergio E Chaigneau,et al. Assessing the causal structure of function. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[50] R. Rumiati. Right, left or both? Brain hemispheres and apraxia of naturalistic actions , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[51] L. Buxbaum,et al. Distinctions between manipulation and function knowledge of objects: evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[52] M. Petrides. Lateral prefrontal cortex: architectonic and functional organization , 2005, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[53] Ueber „subcorticale“ Alexie mit Agraphie und Apraxie , 1903, Deutsche Zeitschrift für Nervenheilkunde.
[54] J. Hermsdörfer,et al. It takes the whole brain to make a cup of coffee: the neuropsychology of naturalistic actions involving technical devices , 2005, Neuropsychologia.
[55] F. Lacquaniti,et al. Cognitive, perceptual and action-oriented representations of falling objects , 2005, Neuropsychologia.
[56] M. Catani,et al. The rises and falls of disconnection syndromes. , 2005, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[57] A. Sirigu,et al. How we interact with objects: learning from brain lesions , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[58] JamesW. Lewis. Cortical Networks Related to Human Use of Tools , 2006, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.
[59] Alan Sunderland,et al. Ideomotor Apraxia And Functional Ability , 2007, Cortex.
[60] S. Frey. What Puts the How in Where? Tool Use and the Divided Visual Streams Hypothesis , 2007, Cortex.
[61] C. Summerfield,et al. An information theoretical approach to prefrontal executive function , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[62] G. Gigli,et al. Degraded Semantic Knowledge And Accurate Object Use , 2007, Cortex.
[63] G. Goldenberg,et al. The Impact of Dysexecutive Syndrome on Use of Tools and Technical Devices , 2007, Cortex.
[64] Claudio Babiloni,et al. Human neural systems for conceptual knowledge of proper object use: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.
[65] Chris Rorden,et al. Improving Lesion-Symptom Mapping , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[66] F. Osiurak,et al. Different constraints on grip selection in brain-damaged patients: Object use versus object transport , 2008, Neuropsychologia.
[67] Tim Shallice,et al. The different neural correlates of action and functional knowledge in semantic memory: an FMRI study. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.
[68] G. Goldenberg,et al. Neuropsychology and behavioral neurology , 2008 .
[69] G. Goldenberg. Apraxia and the parietal lobes , 2009, Neuropsychologia.
[70] F. Osiurak,et al. Unusual use of objects after unilateral brain damage. The technical reasoning model , 2009, Cortex.
[71] Frank Marchak,et al. Constraints for Action Selection: Overhand Versus Underhand Grips , 2018, Attention and Performance XIII.