The Hand Sees Visual Periphery Better Than the Eye: Motor-Dependent Visual Motion Analyses

Information pertaining to visual motion is used in the brain not only for conscious perception but also for various kinds of motor controls. In contrast to the increasing amount of evidence supporting the dissociation of visual processing for action versus perception, it is less clear whether the analysis of visual input is shared for characterizing various motor outputs, which require different kinds of interactions with environments. Here we show that, in human visuomotor control, motion analysis for quick hand control is distinct from that for quick eye control in terms of spatiotemporal analysis and spatial integration. The amplitudes of implicit and quick hand and eye responses induced by visual motion stimuli differently varied with stimulus size and pattern smoothness (e.g., spatial frequency). Surprisingly, the hand response did not decrease even when the visual motion with a coarse pattern was mostly occluded over the visual center, whereas the eye response markedly decreased. Since these contrasts cannot be ascribed to any difference in motor dynamics, they clearly indicate different spatial integration of visual motion for the individual motor systems. Going against the overly unified hierarchical view of visual analysis, our data suggest that visual motion analyses are separately tailored from early levels to individual motor modalities. Namely, the hand and eyes see the external world differently.

[1]  S. Zeki Functional specialisation in the visual cortex of the rhesus monkey , 1978, Nature.

[2]  F. A. Miles,et al.  Short-latency ocular following responses of monkey. I. Dependence on temporospatial properties of visual input. , 1986, Journal of neurophysiology.

[3]  Guy A. Orban,et al.  Similarities and differences in motion processing between the human and macaque brain: evidence from fMRI , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[4]  Gabriel M. Gauthier,et al.  Encoding the position of a flashed visual target after passive body rotations , 1995, Neuroreport.

[5]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Similarity in the response of smooth pursuit and manual tracking to a change in the direction of target motion. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[6]  G. Orban,et al.  Extracting 3D from Motion: Differences in Human and Monkey Intraparietal Cortex , 2002, Science.

[7]  F. Sharp,et al.  Diffusible neurotrophic factors for thalamic neurons are released from target neocortex and non-target cerebellum. , 1990, Progress in brain research.

[8]  Eric Castet,et al.  Parallel Motion Processing for the Initiation of Short-Latency Ocular Following in Humans , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[9]  Shin'ya Nishida,et al.  How motion signals are integrated across frequencies: study on motion perception and ocular following responses using multiple-slit stimuli. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  C. Prablanc,et al.  Large adjustments in visually guided reaching do not depend on vision of the hand or perception of target displacement , 1986, Nature.

[11]  J. Rovamo,et al.  Cortical magnification factor predicts the photopic contrast sensitivity of peripheral vision , 1978, Nature.

[12]  C. Galletti,et al.  The cortical connections of area V6: an occipito‐parietal network processing visual information , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.

[13]  C. Galletti,et al.  ‘Real-motion’ cells in area V3A of macaque visual cortex , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[14]  D. Burr,et al.  Contrast sensitivity at high velocities , 1982, Vision Research.

[15]  C. Galletti,et al.  The cortical visual area V6: brain location and visual topography , 1999, The European journal of neuroscience.

[16]  T. Brandt,et al.  Dependence of visual stabilization of postural sway on the cortical magnification factor of restricted visual fields , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[17]  T. Schenk,et al.  An allocentric rather than perceptual deficit in patient D.F. , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[18]  S. Zeki,et al.  A visuo‐somatomotor pathway through superior parietal cortex in the macaque monkey: cortical connections of areas V6 and V6A , 1998, The European journal of neuroscience.

[19]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Postural readjustments induced by linear motion of visual scenes , 1977, Experimental Brain Research.

[20]  Dennis M. Levi,et al.  Spatial and velocity tuning of processes underlying induced motion , 1984, Vision Research.

[21]  S. Yasui,et al.  Perceived visual motion as effective stimulus to pursuit eye movement system , 1975, Science.

[22]  Grating and plaid chrominance motion influences the suppressed ocular following response. , 1999, Neuroreport.

[23]  E. Brenner,et al.  10 years of illusions. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[24]  K Cheng,et al.  Human cortical regions activated by wide-field visual motion: an H2(15)O PET study. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  Shin'ya Nishida,et al.  Large-Field Visual Motion Directly Induces an Involuntary Rapid Manual Following Response , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  David Whitney,et al.  Motion distorts visual space: shifting the perceived position of remote stationary objects , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[27]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Area V5 of the human brain: evidence from a combined study using positron emission tomography and magnetic resonance imaging. , 1993, Cerebral cortex.

[28]  M. J. Wright,et al.  Visual motion and cortical velocity , 1983, Nature.

[29]  M. Graziano,et al.  Tuning of MST neurons to spiral motions , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[30]  R. Krauzlis,et al.  Shared motion signals for human perceptual decisions and oculomotor actions. , 2003, Journal of vision.

[31]  E. Brenner,et al.  Fast Responses of the Human Hand to Changes in Target Position. , 1997, Journal of motor behavior.

[32]  M. Goodale,et al.  Visual control of reaching movements without vision of the limb , 1986, Experimental Brain Research.

[33]  A. T. Smith,et al.  Estimating receptive field size from fMRI data in human striate and extrastriate visual cortex. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[34]  J. B. Levitt,et al.  Functional properties of neurons in macaque area V3. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[35]  H. Komatsu,et al.  Relation of cortical areas MT and MST to pursuit eye movements. I. Localization and visual properties of neurons. , 1988, Journal of neurophysiology.

[36]  L. Jakobson,et al.  A neurological dissociation between perceiving objects and grasping them , 1991, Nature.

[37]  R. L. Valois,et al.  Vernier acuity with stationary moving Gabors , 1991, Vision Research.

[38]  Andreas Bartels,et al.  Human Areas V3A and V6 Compensate for Self-Induced Planar Visual Motion , 2012, Neuron.

[39]  R. M. Siegel,et al.  Corticocortical connections of anatomically and physiologically defined subdivisions within the inferior parietal lobule , 1990, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[40]  A. Cowey,et al.  Topography of the retina and striate cortex and its relationship to visual acuity in rhesus monkeys and squirrel monkeys , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[41]  C. Galletti,et al.  Spatial tuning of reaching activity in the medial parieto‐occipital cortex (area V6A) of macaque monkey , 2005, The European journal of neuroscience.

[42]  Aya Takemura,et al.  Deficits in Short-Latency Tracking Eye Movements after Chemical Lesions in Monkey Cortical Areas MT and MST , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[43]  O. Braddick,et al.  Brain Areas Sensitive to Coherent Visual Motion , 2001, Perception.

[44]  Shin'ya Nishida,et al.  Spatiotemporal Tuning of Rapid Interactions between Visual-Motion Analysis and Reaching Movement , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[45]  G. Orban,et al.  Motion-responsive regions of the human brain , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[46]  P. Dixon,et al.  A step and a hop on the Müller-Lyer: illusion effects on lower-limb movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[47]  D C Van Essen,et al.  Information processing in the primate visual system: an integrated systems perspective. , 1992, Science.

[48]  Hiroaki Gomi,et al.  Spatial coincidence of intentional actions modulates an implicit visuomotor control. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[49]  M. Fahle,et al.  Grasping Visual Illusions: No Evidence for a Dissociation Between Perception and Action , 2000, Psychological science.

[50]  Velia Cardin,et al.  Sensitivity of human visual and vestibular cortical regions to egomotion-compatible visual stimulation. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[51]  Stephen G. Lisberger,et al.  Directional Anisotropies Reveal a Functional Segregation of Visual Motion Processing for Perception and Action , 2003, Neuron.

[52]  J. Vercher,et al.  Failure to Update the Egocentric Representation of the Visual Space Through Labyrinthine Signal , 1995, Brain and Cognition.

[53]  Robert H. Wurtz,et al.  Multiple temporal components of optic flow responses in MST neurons , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[54]  D. Burr,et al.  Spatial summation properties of directionally selective mechanisms in human vision. , 1991, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[55]  J. B. Levitt,et al.  Circuits for Local and Global Signal Integration in Primary Visual Cortex , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[56]  J. Dichgans,et al.  Differential effects of central versus peripheral vision on egocentric and exocentric motion perception , 1973, Experimental Brain Research.

[57]  J. Hennig,et al.  The Processing of First- and Second-Order Motion in Human Visual Cortex Assessed by Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[58]  David Whitney,et al.  The influence of visual motion on fast reaching movements to a stationary object , 2003, Nature.

[59]  C. Galletti,et al.  Wide-Field Retinotopy Defines Human Cortical Visual Area V6 , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[60]  A. Dale,et al.  Functional Analysis of V3A and Related Areas in Human Visual Cortex , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[61]  S. Yamane,et al.  Neural activity in cortical area MST of alert monkey during ocular following responses. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[62]  Visual pop-out in infancy: Effects of set-size on the latency of their eye movements , 2010 .

[63]  Luc Proteau,et al.  Effects of stationary and moving textured backgrounds on the visuo-oculo-manual tracking in humans , 1995, Vision Research.

[64]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[65]  B. Wandell,et al.  Visual field maps, population receptive field sizes, and visual field coverage in the human MT+ complex. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[66]  Kenji Kawano,et al.  Eye movements in response to dichoptic motion: evidence for a parallel-hierarchical structure of visual motion processing in primates. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[67]  C Galletti,et al.  Superior area 6 afferents from the superior parietal lobule in the macaque monkey , 1998, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[68]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Pathways for motion analysis: Cortical connections of the medial superior temporal and fundus of the superior temporal visual areas in the macaque , 1990, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[69]  K. D. De Valois,et al.  Vernier acuity with stationary moving Gabors. , 1991, Vision research.

[70]  Nicholas J. Priebe,et al.  Estimating Target Speed from the Population Response in Visual Area MT , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[71]  Julian J Tramper,et al.  Visuomotor coordination is different for different directions in three-dimensional space. , 2011, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[72]  H. Gomi Implicit online corrections of reaching movements , 2008, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[73]  M. Goodale,et al.  Visual control of reaching movements without vision of the limb , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[74]  D C Van Essen,et al.  Functional properties of neurons in middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey. I. Selectivity for stimulus direction, speed, and orientation. , 1983, Journal of neurophysiology.

[75]  L S Stone,et al.  Motion coherence affects human perception and pursuit similarly , 2000, Visual Neuroscience.

[76]  W. Newsome,et al.  A selective impairment of motion perception following lesions of the middle temporal visual area (MT) , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[77]  G. Orban,et al.  Many areas in the human brain respond to visual motion. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[78]  Hiroaki Gomi,et al.  Implicit Visuomotor Processing for Quick Online Reactions Is Robust against Aging , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.