The development of infant discrimination of affect in multimodal and unimodal stimulation: The role of intersensory redundancy.

This research examined the developmental course of infants' ability to perceive affect in bimodal (audiovisual) and unimodal (auditory and visual) displays of a woman speaking. According to the intersensory redundancy hypothesis (L. E. Bahrick, R. Lickliter, & R. Flom, 2004), detection of amodal properties is facilitated in multimodal stimulation and attenuated in unimodal stimulation. Later in development, however, attention becomes more flexible, and amodal properties can be perceived in both multimodal and unimodal stimulation. The authors tested these predictions by assessing 3-, 4-, 5-, and 7-month-olds' discrimination of affect. Results demonstrated that in bimodal stimulation, discrimination of affect emerged by 4 months and remained stable across age. However, in unimodal stimulation, detection of affect emerged gradually, with sensitivity to auditory stimulation emerging at 5 months and visual stimulation at 7 months. Further temporal synchrony between faces and voices was necessary for younger infants' discrimination of affect. Across development, infants first perceive affect in multimodal stimulation through detecting amodal properties, and later their perception of affect is extended to unimodal auditory and visual stimulation. Implications for social development, including joint attention and social referencing, are considered.

[1]  Sidney S. Simon,et al.  Merging of the Senses , 2008, Front. Neurosci..

[2]  D. Muir,et al.  Gaze-following : its development and significance , 2007 .

[3]  R. Lickliter,et al.  Up Versus Down: The Role of Intersensory Redundancy in the Development of Infants' Sensitivity to the Orientation of Moving Objects. , 2006, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[4]  L. Bahrick,et al.  Infant Perception of Object-Affect Relations , 2005 .

[5]  Robert Lickliter,et al.  Infants’ perception of rhythm and tempo in unimodal and multimodal stimulation: A developmental test of the intersensory redundancy hypothesis , 2004, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[6]  R. Lickliter,et al.  Intersensory Redundancy Guides the Development of Selective Attention, Perception, and Cognition in Infancy , 2004 .

[7]  T. Striano,et al.  Is visual reference necessary? Contributions of facial versus vocal cues in 12-month-olds' social referencing behavior. , 2004, Developmental science.

[8]  R. Lickliter,et al.  Intersensory Redundancy Enhances Memory in Bobwhite Quail Embryos , 2004 .

[9]  Gedeon O. Deák,et al.  Nine-month-olds' shared visual attention as a function of gesture and object location. , 2004 .

[10]  David J. Lewkowicz,et al.  Serial order processing in human infants and the role of multisensory redundancy , 2004, Cognitive Processing.

[11]  R. Lickliter,et al.  Perceptual Development and the Origins of Multisensory Responsiveness. , 2004 .

[12]  R. Lickliter,et al.  Effects of redundant and nonredundant bimodal sensory stimulation on heart rate in bobwhite quail embryos. , 2003, Developmental psychobiology.

[13]  D. L. Mumme,et al.  The infant as onlooker: learning from emotional reactions observed in a television scenario. , 2003, Child development.

[14]  Robert Lickliter,et al.  Intersensory redundancy facilitates discrimination of tempo in 3-month-old infants. , 2002, Developmental psychobiology.

[15]  S. A. Rose,et al.  Processing speed in the 1st year of life: a longitudinal study of preterm and full-term infants. , 2002, Developmental psychology.

[16]  H. Wellman,et al.  Infants' ability to connect gaze and emotional expression to intentional action , 2002, Cognition.

[17]  Marc H. Bornstein,et al.  Development in infancy : an introduction , 2002 .

[18]  R. Lickliter,et al.  Intersensory redundancy facilitates prenatal perceptual learning in bobwhite quail (Colinus virginianus) embryos. , 2002, Developmental psychology.

[19]  R. Lickliter,et al.  Intersensory redundancy guides early perceptual and cognitive development. , 2002, Advances in child development and behavior.

[20]  A. Walker-Andrews,et al.  Mothers, fathers, and infants: the role of person familiarity and parental involvement in infants' perception of emotion expressions. , 2002, Child development.

[21]  A. Walker-Andrews,et al.  Peekaboo: a new look at infants' perception of emotion expressions. , 2001, Developmental psychology.

[22]  R. Lickliter,et al.  The Salience of Multimodal Sensory Stimulation in Early Development: Implications for the Issue of Ecological Validity. , 2001, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[23]  L. Bahrick,et al.  Perceiving the Real World: Infants' Detection of and Memory for Social Information. , 2001, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[24]  L. Bahrick,et al.  Increasing specificity in perceptual development: infants' detection of nested levels of multimodal stimulation. , 2001, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[25]  A. Walker-Andrews,et al.  The role of person familiarity in young infants' perception of emotional expressions. , 2001, Child development.

[26]  D. Lewkowicz Infants' perception of the audible, visible, and bimodal attributes of multimodal syllables. , 2000, Child development.

[27]  A. Pick,et al.  Effects of gesture and target on 12- and 18-month-olds' joint visual attention to objects in front of or behind them. , 2000, Developmental psychology.

[28]  R. Lickliter,et al.  Intersensory redundancy guides attentional selectivity and perceptual learning in infancy. , 2000, Developmental psychology.

[29]  R. Lickliter,et al.  The development of infant intersensory perception: advantages of a comparative convergent-operations approach. , 2000, Psychological bulletin.

[30]  D. Lewkowicz,et al.  The development of intersensory temporal perception: an epigenetic systems/limitations view. , 2000, Psychological bulletin.

[31]  Michael Morales,et al.  Following the direction of gaze and language development in 6-month-olds , 1998 .

[32]  M. Arterberry,et al.  The Cradle of Knowledge: Development of Perception in Infancy , 1998 .

[33]  C. Moore,et al.  The origins of joint visual attention in infants. , 1998, Developmental psychology.

[34]  A. Walker-Andrews Infants' perception of expressive behaviors: differentiation of multimodal information. , 1997, Psychological bulletin.

[35]  C. Moore,et al.  The role of movement in the development of joint visual attention , 1997 .

[36]  D. L. Mumme,et al.  Infants' responses to facial and vocal emotional signals in a social referencing paradigm. , 1996, Child development.

[37]  D. Lewkowicz Infants' Response to the Audible and Visible Properties of the Human Face: 1. Role of Lexical-Syntactic Content, Temporal Synchrony, Gender, and Manner of Speech , 1996 .

[38]  Josef P. Rauschecker,et al.  The Development of Intersensory Perception: Comparative Perspectives , 1996, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[39]  D. Lewkowicz Perception of auditory-visual temporal synchrony in human infants. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[40]  Lorraine E. Bahrick,et al.  Amodal relations: The basis for intermodal perception and learning in infancy. , 1994 .

[41]  Philip J. Dunham,et al.  Joint-Attentional States and Lexical Acquisition at 18 Months. , 1993 .

[42]  J. M. Serrano,et al.  Visual discrimination and recognition of facial expressions of anger, fear, and surprise in 4- to 6-month-old infants. , 1992, Developmental psychobiology.

[43]  A. Pick,et al.  Intermodal perception of happy and angry expressive behaviors by seven-month-old infants. , 1992, Child development.

[44]  David J. Lewkowicz,et al.  Infants' response to temporally based intersensory equivalence: The effect of synchronous sounds on visual preferences for moving stimuli , 1992 .

[45]  L. Bahrick Infants' perceptual differentiation of amodal and modality-specific audio-visual relations. , 1992, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[46]  A. Walker-Andrews,et al.  Infants' discrimination of vocal expressions: Contributions of auditory and visual information , 1991 .

[47]  R. Kestenbaum,et al.  The recognition and categorization of upright and inverted emotional expressions by 7-month-old infants , 1990 .

[48]  S Hale,et al.  A global developmental trend in cognitive processing speed. , 1990, Child development.

[49]  G. Turkewitz,et al.  Dynamic organization of intersensory function. , 1989, Canadian journal of psychology.

[50]  T. Walden,et al.  The development of social referencing. , 1988, Child development.

[51]  C. Nelson,et al.  The Categorical Representation of Facial Expressions by 7-Month-Old Infants , 2011 .

[52]  A. J. Caron,et al.  Infant discrimination of naturalistic emotional expressions: the role of face and voice. , 1988, Child development.

[53]  L. Bahrick,et al.  Intermodal learning in infancy: learning on the basis of two kinds of invariant relations in audible and visible events. , 1988, Child development.

[54]  E. Gibson Exploratory behavior in the development of perceiving, acting, and the acquiring of knowledge. , 1988 .

[55]  Megan R. Gunnar,et al.  The effects of maternal positive, neutral and negative affective communications on infant responses to new toys , 1987 .

[56]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Joint attention and early language. , 1986, Child development.

[57]  A. Walker-Andrews Intermodal perception of expressive behaviors: Relation of eye and voice? , 1986 .

[58]  A. Walker-Andrews,et al.  Auditory-visual perception of changing distance by human infants. , 1985, Child development.

[59]  A. J. Caron,et al.  Do infants see emotional expressions in static faces? , 1985, Child development.

[60]  J. Campos,et al.  Maternal emotional signaling: Its effect on the visual cliff behavior of 1-year-olds. , 1985 .

[61]  Elizabeth S Spelke,et al.  Perception of Moving, Sounding Objects by Four-Month-Old Infants , 1983, Perception.

[62]  Wendy S. Grolnick,et al.  Discrimination of vocal expressions by young infants , 1983 .

[63]  Lorraine E. Bahrick,et al.  Infants' perception of substance and temporal synchrony in multimodal events* , 1983 .

[64]  S. Rose Differential rates of visual information processing in full-term and preterm infants. , 1983, Child development.

[65]  B. Bertenthal,et al.  The partial-lag design: A method for controlling spontaneous regression in the infant-control habituation paradigm* , 1983 .

[66]  G Turkewitz,et al.  Limitations on input as a basis for neural organization and perceptual development: a preliminary theoretical statement. , 1982, Developmental psychobiology.

[67]  A. Walker,et al.  Intermodal perception of expressive behaviors by human infants. , 1982, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[68]  C. Nelson,et al.  Recognition of facial expressions by seven-month-old infants. , 1979, Child development.

[69]  J. Gibson The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1979 .

[70]  J Kagan Visual attention, auditory stimulation, and language discrimination in young infants. Commentary. , 1974, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[71]  J. Fagan,et al.  Infant recognition memory: the effects of length of familiarization and type of discrimination task. , 1974, Child development.

[72]  M. Shishuku [Visual discrimination]. , 1974, Kokubyo Gakkai zasshi. The Journal of the Stomatological Society, Japan.

[73]  F. D. Horowitz,et al.  An Infant Control Procedure for Studying Infant Visual Fixations. , 1972 .

[74]  Lester R. Aronson,et al.  The biopsychology of development , 1971 .

[75]  J. Gibson The Senses Considered As Perceptual Systems , 1967 .

[76]  L. Frank The Society for Research in Child Development , 1935 .

[77]  I. Sigel,et al.  HANDBOOK OF CHILD PSYCHOLOGY , 2006 .