Social Sounds: Vocal Learning and Development of Mammal and Bird Calls

First, we hear a rustle in the shrubs, and then a bird whose call sounds like “where are you?” From a short distance away, another bird gives a slightly different call: “oh, where are you?” For simplicity, we will call these WAY calls. In answer, several small gray and white wrens fly toward the first caller, giving WAY calls that sound virtually identical to the first caller’s. At the same time, a small flock flies toward the second caller, giving their own WAY calls. A skirmish follows, with much calling on either side. Careful listening picks up distinct WAY calls throughout and another staccato call, which turns out to be a duet. When the dust settles, the small flocks move off in different directions, territory boundaries apparently stable for the day. The birds are stripe-backed wrens, and this species lives in stable groups composed of a dominant pair and their offspring, who cooperate to care for the pair’s brood (Rabenold 1984). Group mates also cooperate to defend their joint territory. The dominant pair advertises boundaries by singing duets, but when a neighbor intrudes, WAY calls provide a means of identifying group mates. All males in a group share a repertoire of about 12 WAY calls that they use both to maintain contact among the group and to identify themselves in territory disputes. Female group members have their own shared repertoire of WAY calls (Price 1998a).WAY calls indicate group membership—a kind of badge to indicate “I belong to this group.” All stripe-backed wrens live in social groups, so belonging to a group is critical. But, how do group members come to share calls? Most group mates are closely related, so call similarity based on genetic similarity is one possibility. Yet, calls are passed down along sex-specific lines, and a genetic mechanism for this is difficult to envision. Instead, it seems that calls are learned—sons copy their fathers and daughters copy their mothers, and do so with remarkable precision. Like a surname passed on from father to son, or mother to daughter (Price 1998a), calls convey family identity (Price 1999). The patterns observed suggest that call sharing is essential to call function. Although most males in a group are closely related, occasionally

[1]  M. Dahlheim,et al.  Low genetic variation among killer whales (Orcinus orca) in the eastern north Pacific and genetic differentiation between foraging specialists. , 1998, The Journal of heredity.

[2]  A. Skutch,et al.  Studies of tropical American birds , 1972 .

[3]  Volker Bernt Deeke Stability and change of killer whale (Orcinus orca) dialects , 1998 .

[4]  R. Seyfarth,et al.  Social influences on vocal development: Some general features of vocal development in nonhuman primates , 1997 .

[5]  S. O’Brien,et al.  Influence of seasonal migration on geographic distribution of mitochondrial DNA haplotypes in humpback whales , 1990, Nature.

[6]  Paul A. Garber,et al.  Diet, Foraging Patterns, and Resource Defense in a Mixed Species Troop of Saguinus Mystax and Saguinus Fuscicollis in Amazonian Peru , 1988 .

[7]  W. Steiner,et al.  Species-specific differences in pure tonal whistle vocalizations of five western North Atlantic dolphin species , 1981, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[8]  D. Chivers Malayan Forest Primates: Ten Years’ Study in Tropical Rain Forest , 1980 .

[9]  W. C. Cummings,et al.  Song of the humpback whale — Population comparisons , 1981, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[10]  R. Connor,et al.  Seasonal changes in the stability of male-male bonds in Indian Ocean bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp.) , 1995 .

[11]  Peter L. Tyack,et al.  MALE COMPETITION IN LARGE GROUPS OF WINTERING HUMPBACK WHALES , 1983 .

[12]  H. Gibbs Cultural evolution of male song types in Darwin's medium ground finches, Geospiza fortis , 1990, Animal Behaviour.

[13]  P. Tyack,et al.  Interactions between singing Hawaiian humpback whales and conspecifics nearby , 1981, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[14]  P. Slater,et al.  The effect of new recruits on the flock specific call of budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus) , 1999 .

[15]  A. F. McBride,et al.  Observations on Pregnancy, Parturition, and Postnatal Behavior in the Bottlenose Dolphin , 1951 .

[16]  U. Gyllensten,et al.  Global matrilineal population structure in sperm whales as indicated by mitochondrial DNA sequences , 1998, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[17]  P. Marler,et al.  Individuality in a long-range vocalization of wild chimpanzees. , 2010, Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie.

[18]  Robert D. McCauley,et al.  Geograpmc Variation in South Pacific Humpback Whale Songs , 1998 .

[19]  C. Weise,et al.  A complex call of the black-capped chickadee (Parus atricapillus). I. Microgeographic variation , 1984 .

[20]  D. Weary,et al.  Great tits classify songs by individual voice characteristics , 1992, Animal Behaviour.

[21]  C. Snowdon,et al.  Acoustic adaptation in pygmy marmoset contact calls: Locational cues vary with distances between conspecifics , 1981, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[22]  J. Randall Modification of footdrumming signatures by kangaroo rats: changing territories and gaining new neighbours , 1995, Animal Behaviour.

[23]  E. Zimmermann Loud Calls in Nocturnal Prosimians: Structure, Evolution and Ontogeny , 1995 .

[24]  H. Rothe,et al.  Infant survival and number of helpers in captive groups of common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) , 1993, American journal of primatology.

[25]  H. Gouzoules,et al.  Sex differences in the acquisition of communicative competence by pigtail macaques (Macaca nemestrina) , 1989, American journal of primatology.

[26]  F. Waal,et al.  Coalitions and alliances in humans and other animals , 1993 .

[27]  E. D. Brown,et al.  Analysis of warble song of the budgerigar Melopsittacus undulatus , 1992 .

[28]  C. Snowdon,et al.  Demography, group composition, and dispersal in wild cotton‐top tamarin (Saguinus oedipus) groups , 1996, American journal of primatology.

[29]  Gerald S. Wilkinson,et al.  Birth synchrony in greater spear‐nosed bats (Phyllostomus hastatus) , 2001 .

[30]  G. Wilkinson,et al.  Population genetic structure and vocal dialects in an amazon parrot , 2001, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[31]  M. Ficken,et al.  Long-term persistence of a culturally transmitted vocalization of the black-capped chickadee , 1995, Animal Behaviour.

[32]  S. Brauth,et al.  Effects of deafening on the development of nestling and juvenile vocalizations in budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus). , 1999, Journal of comparative psychology.

[33]  H. Whitehead,et al.  Group-specific dialects and geographical variation in coda repertoire in South Pacific sperm whales , 1997, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[34]  G. Striedter,et al.  Male vocal imitation produces call convergence during pair bonding in budgerigars, Melopsittacus undulatus , 2000, Animal Behaviour.

[35]  A. Grafen Do animals really recognize kin? , 1990, Animal Behaviour.

[36]  P. Parker,et al.  Facultative dispersal by juvenile males in the cooperative stripe-backed wren , 1995 .

[37]  D. Westneat,et al.  A GENETIC AND BEHAVIORAL ANALYSIS OF MATE CHOICE AND SONG NEIGHBORHOODS IN INDIGO BUNTINGS , 1988, Evolution; international journal of organic evolution.

[38]  R. Wrangham,et al.  Does learning affect the structure of vocalizations in chimpanzees? , 1999, Animal Behaviour.

[39]  P. Slater,et al.  Vocal Learning in Mammals , 1997 .

[40]  M. A. Cunningham,et al.  The Biology of Bird-Song Dialects , 1985, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[41]  A. Hoelzel Foraging behaviour and social group dynamics in Puget Sound killer whales , 1993, Animal Behaviour.

[42]  Acoustic communication in bottlenose dolphins. , 1993 .

[43]  A. King,et al.  Female visual displays affect the development of male song in the cowbird , 1988, Nature.

[44]  Genetic differentiation among matrilines in social groups of rhesus monkeys , 1981, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[45]  P. Handford Trill rate dialects in the Rufous-collared Sparrow, Zonotrichia capensis, in northwestern Argentina , 1988 .

[46]  Jill M. Trainer Behavioral Associations of Song Types during Aggressive Interactions among Male Yellow-Rumped Caciques , 1987 .

[47]  E. D. Brown,et al.  SONG SHARING IN A GROUP-LIVING SONGBIRD, THE AUSTRALIAN MAGPIE, GYMNORHINA TIBICEN. PART III. SEX SPECIFICITY AND INDIVIDUAL SPECIFICITY OF VOCAL PARTS IN COMMUNAL CHORUS AND DUET SONGS , 1991 .

[48]  T. Kunz,et al.  Ontogeny of vocal signals in the little brown bat, Myotis lucifugus , 1997, Animal Behaviour.

[49]  M. Hauser The Evolution of Nonhuman Primate Vocalizations: Effects of Phylogeny, Body Weight, and Social Context , 1993, The American Naturalist.

[50]  J. Randall Discrimination of footdrumming signatures by kangaroo rats, Dipodomys spectabilis , 1994, Animal Behaviour.

[51]  John W. Pepper,et al.  Sex Differences in Patterns of Association Among Indian Ocean Bottlenose Dolphins , 1992 .

[52]  T. Wright,et al.  Pair duets in the yellow-naped amazon (psittaciformes : Amazona auropalliata) : Responses to playbacks of different dialects , 2001 .

[53]  E. D. Brown,et al.  SONG SHARING IN A GROUP-LIVING SONGBIRD, THE AUSTRALIAN MAGPIE. PART II. VOCAL SHARING BETWEEN TERRITORIAL NEIGHBORS, WITHIN AND BETWEEN GEOGRAPHIC REGIONS, AND BETWEEN SEXES , 1988 .

[54]  R. Andrew,et al.  Evolution of Intelligence and Vocal Mimicking: Studies of large-brained mammals promise to elucidate some problems of human evolution. , 1962, Science.

[55]  Hanne Strager Pod-specific call repertoires and compound calls of killer whales, Orcinus orca Linnaeus, 1758, in the waters of northern Norway , 1995 .

[56]  Gareth Jones,et al.  Echolocation calls of bats are influenced by maternal effects and change over a lifetime , 1993, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[57]  R. Rübsamen,et al.  Ontogenesis of tonotopy in inferior colliculus of a hipposiderid bat reveals postnatal shift in frequency-place code , 1989, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[58]  Jeremy M. V. Rayner,et al.  Recent Advances in the Study of Bats , 1988 .

[59]  M. Fenton,et al.  Can echolocation calls provide information about group identity in the little brown bat (Myotis lucifugus) , 1996 .

[60]  D. K. Caldwell,et al.  Individualized Whistle Contours in Bottle-nosed Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) , 1965, Nature.

[61]  J. C. Fentress Simpler networks and behavior , 1976 .

[62]  S. Katona,et al.  Whale migration record , 1990, Nature.

[63]  D. Rendall,et al.  Vocal recognition of individuals and kin in free-ranging rhesus monkeys , 1996, Animal Behaviour.

[64]  Anil Kumar,et al.  Acoustic communication in birds , 2003 .

[65]  J. Randall Individual footdrumming signatures in banner-tailed kangaroo rats Dipodomys spectabilis , 1989, Animal Behaviour.

[66]  P. Marler,et al.  Selection-based learning in bird song development. , 1994, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[67]  R. Bowman,et al.  Occurrence, population composition, and habitat use of humpback whales in Samana Bay, Dominican Republic , 1994 .

[68]  S. I. Rothstein,et al.  Vocal Dialects and Their Possible Relation to Honest Status Signalling in the Brown-Headed Cowbird , 1987 .

[69]  R. Busnel,et al.  Animal Sonar Systems , 1980, NATO Advanced Study Institutes Series.

[70]  T. G. Smith,et al.  Underwater vocalizations of the bearded seal (Erignathus barbatus) , 1989 .

[71]  Jeanette A. Thomas,et al.  Geographic variation in the underwater vocalizations of Weddell seals (Leptonychotes weddelli) from Palmer Peninsula and McMurdo Sound, Antarctica , 1983 .

[72]  W. E. Evans,et al.  Observations on Scouting Behavior and Associated Sound Production by the Pacific Bottlenosed Porpoise (Tursiops gilli Dall) , 1962 .

[73]  J. W. Boughman,et al.  Vocal learning by greater spear–nosed bats , 1998, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[74]  N Suga,et al.  Syntax processing by auditory cortical neurons in the FM-FM area of the mustached bat Pteronotus parnellii. , 1997, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[75]  A. Rylands,et al.  Marmosets and tamarins : systematics, behaviour, and ecology , 1993 .

[76]  Hans-Ulrich Schnitzler,et al.  Performance of Airborne Animal Sonar Systems: I. Microchiroptera , 1980 .

[77]  K. N. Rabenold Cooperative Enhancement Of Reproductive Success in Tropical Wren Societies , 1984 .

[78]  R. Connor,et al.  Two levels of alliance formation among male bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp.). , 1992, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[79]  D. Ploog,et al.  Ontogeny of squirrel monkey calls under normal conditions and under acoustic isolation. , 1973, Behaviour.

[80]  D. K. Caldwell,et al.  The Whistle of the Atlantic Bottlenosed Dolphin (Tursiops truncatus)—Ontogeny , 1979 .

[81]  J. Buchwald,et al.  Individual differences in threat calls of northern elephant seal bulls , 1981, Animal Behaviour.

[82]  Melba C. Caldwell,et al.  10 – Review of the Signature-Whistle Hypothesis for the Atlantic Bottlenose Dolphin , 1990 .

[83]  C. Peres,et al.  Riverine barriers and gene flow in Amazonian saddle-back tamarins. , 1996, Folia primatologica; international journal of primatology.

[84]  J. Buchwald,et al.  Vocalizations of Northern Elephant Seal Bulls: Development of Adult Call Characteristics during Puberty , 1986 .

[85]  J. Mitani,et al.  Contexts and social correlates of long-distance calling by male chimpanzees , 1993, Animal Behaviour.

[86]  D. Griffin,et al.  Listening in the Dark , 1959 .

[87]  J. Price Recognition of family-specific calls in stripe-backed wrens , 1999, Animal Behaviour.

[88]  J. Ford,et al.  Acoustic behaviour of resident killer whales (Orcinus orca) off Vancouver Island, British Columbia , 1989 .

[89]  R. Wanker,et al.  Discrimination of different social companions in spectacled parrotlets (Forpus conspicillatus): evidence for individual vocal recognition , 1998, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[90]  P. Marler,et al.  Dialects in wild chimpanzees? , 1992, American journal of primatology.

[91]  P. Tyack,et al.  Whistle repertoires of two bottlenosed dolphins, Tursiops truncatus: mimicry of signature whistles? , 1986, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[92]  J. A. Baker,et al.  Recognition Characteristics in Covey Dialects of Bobwhite Quail , 1982 .

[93]  R. W. Ficken,et al.  Dialects in a call associated with pair interactions in the black-capped chickadee , 1985 .

[94]  S. Palumbi,et al.  Contrasting population structure from nuclear intron sequences and mtDNA of humpback whales. , 1994, Molecular biology and evolution.

[95]  M. Ficken,et al.  Associations among members of a black-capped chickadee flock , 1981, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[96]  M. Morrice,et al.  Microgeographic variation and songs in the underwater vocalisation repertoire of the Weddell seal (Leptonychotes weddellii) from the Vestfold Hills, Antarctica , 1994, Polar Biology.

[97]  E. Kalko,et al.  Insect pursuit, prey capture and echolocation in pipestirelle bats (Microchiroptera) , 1995, Animal Behaviour.

[98]  E. Mirsky Song Divergence in Hummingbird and Junco Populations on Guadalupe Island , 1976 .

[99]  R. H. Wiley Song Groups in a Singing Assembly of Little Hermits , 1971 .

[100]  John R. Krebs,et al.  Repertoires and Geographical Variation in Bird Song , 1980 .

[101]  B. L. Le Boeuf,et al.  Dialects in Elephant Seals , 1969, Science.

[102]  B. Grant,et al.  CULTURAL INHERITANCE OF SONG AND ITS ROLE IN THE EVOLUTION OF DARWIN'S FINCHES , 1996, Evolution; international journal of organic evolution.

[103]  K. Esser,et al.  Behavioral auditory thresholds in neonate lesser spear-nosed bats,Phyllostomus discolor , 1990, The Science of Nature.

[104]  E. Wyndham Environment and food of the budgerigar Melopsittacus undulatus , 1980 .

[105]  H. Whitehead,et al.  Patterns of kinship in groups of free-living sperm whales (Physeter macrocephalus) revealed by multiple molecular genetic analyses. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[106]  D. Chivers,et al.  Long-Term Changes in Behaviour , 1980 .

[107]  Robin I. M. Dunbar,et al.  Evolution and ecology of macaque societies , 1996 .

[108]  Diana Reiss,et al.  Whistle contour development in captive-born infant bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus): Role of learning. , 1995 .

[109]  C. Snowdon,et al.  Predator recognition in cotton‐top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus) , 1990 .

[110]  Louis M. Herman,et al.  Vocal mimicry of computer-generated sounds and vocal labeling of objects by a bottlenosed dolphin, Tursiops truncatus. , 1984, Journal of comparative psychology.

[111]  S. Green,et al.  Dialects in Japanese monkeys: vocal learning and cultural transmission of locale-specific vocal behavior? , 2010, Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie.

[112]  Diana Reiss,et al.  The fallacy of ‘signature whistles’ in bottlenose dolphins: a comparative perspective of ‘signature information’ in animal vocalizations , 2001, Animal Behaviour.

[113]  J. Ford,et al.  Vocal traditions among resident killer whales (Orcinus orca) in coastal waters of British Columbia , 1991 .

[114]  G. McCracken,et al.  Paternity and Genetic Heterogeneity in the Polygynous Bat, Phyllostomus hastatus , 1977, Science.

[115]  John R. Meyer,et al.  Juvenile Dispersal and Development of Site-Fidelity in the Black-Capped Chickadee , 1979 .

[116]  B. L. Boeuf,et al.  Dialects of northern elephant seals, Mirounga angustirostris: Origin and reliability , 1974 .

[117]  S. Boinski,et al.  Chuck Vocalizations of Wild Female Squirrel Monkeys (Saimiri sciureus) Contain Information on Caller Identity and Foraging Activity , 1997, International Journal of Primatology.

[118]  C. Snowdon,et al.  Individual recognition of contact calls by pygmy marmosets , 1980, Animal Behaviour.

[119]  J. Newman,et al.  Individuality and stability of isolation peeps in squirrel monkeys , 1979, Animal Behaviour.

[120]  R. Dooling,et al.  Vocal plasticity in budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus): evidence for social factors in the learning of contact calls. , 1994, Journal of comparative psychology.

[121]  B. Brockway Ethological Studies of the Budgerigar (Melopsittacus Undulatus): Non-Reproductive Behavior , 1964 .

[122]  J. Habersetzer,et al.  Ontogeny of sounds in the echolocating batHipposideros speoris , 1986, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[123]  R. Dooling,et al.  Effects of deafening on the contact call of the budgerigar, Melopsittacus undulatus , 1987, Animal Behaviour.

[124]  A. M. Elowson,et al.  Infant 'Babbling' in a Nonhuman Primate: Complex Vocal Sequences with Repeated Call Types , 1998 .

[125]  N. Masataka,et al.  Vocal learning of Japanese and rhesus monkeys , 1989 .

[126]  J. Mann,et al.  Use of signature whistles during separations and reunions by wild bottlenose dolphin mothers and infants , 1993, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[127]  G. Wilkinson,et al.  Social calls coordinate foraging in greater spear-nosed bats , 1998, Animal Behaviour.

[128]  D. Cheney Interactions and relationships between groups , 1987 .

[129]  K. Esser Audio-vocal learning in a non-human mammal: the lesser spear-nosed bat Phyllostomus discolor. , 1994, Neuroreport.

[130]  J. Calambokidis,et al.  Population structure of nuclear and mitochondrial DNA variation among humpback whales in the North Pacific , 1998, Molecular ecology.

[131]  C. Snowdon,et al.  Population recognition of infant isolation peeps in the squirrel monkey , 1985, Animal Behaviour.

[132]  Randall S. Wells,et al.  Sex difference in signature whistle production of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops truncates , 1995, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[133]  J. Pepper,et al.  Whistle Convergence among Allied Male Bottlenose Dolphins (Delphinidae, Tursiops sp.) , 1999 .

[134]  R. Rübsamen Ontogenesis of the echolocation system in the rufous horseshoe bat,Rhinolophus rouxi (Audition and vocalization in early postnatal development) , 1987, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[135]  E. Gould Mechanisms of mammalian auditory communication , 1983 .

[136]  D. Kroodsma,et al.  Ecology and evolution of acoustic communication in birds , 1997 .

[137]  Uwe Jürgens,et al.  Current Topics in Primate Vocal Communication , 1995, Springer US.

[138]  Robert J Dooling,et al.  Perceptual organization of acoustic stimuli by budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus): III. Contact calls. , 1988, Journal of comparative psychology.

[139]  C. S. Baker,et al.  DIVERSITY AND DISTRIBUTION OF MITOCHONDRIAL DNA LINEAGES AMONG HUMPBACK WHALES, MEGAPTERA NOVAEANGLIAE, IN THE MEXICAN PACIFIC OCEAN , 1995 .

[140]  P. Slater,et al.  The different roles of social learning in vocal communication , 2000, Animal Behaviour.

[141]  M. Parr,et al.  Parrots: A Guide to Parrots of the World , 2006 .

[142]  J. Calambokidis,et al.  Abundant mitochondrial DNA variation and world-wide population structure in humpback whales. , 1993, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[143]  P. Slater,et al.  Bird song learning: causes and consequences , 1989 .

[144]  Robert W. Ficken,et al.  VOCAL REPERTOIRE OF THE BLACK-CAPPED CHICKADEE , 1978 .

[145]  A. M. Elowson,et al.  `Babbling' and social context in infant monkeys: parallels to human infants , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[146]  N Suga,et al.  Facilitative responses to species-specific calls in cortical FM-FM neurons of the mustached bat. , 1996, Neuroreport.

[147]  David S. Jacobs,et al.  Variation in the echolocation calls of the hoary bat (Lasiurus cinereus) : influence of body size, habitat structure, and geographic location , 1999 .

[148]  S. Nowicki,et al.  Vocal plasticity in captive black-capped chickadees: the acoustic basis and rate of call convergence , 1989, Animal Behaviour.

[149]  E W Hansen,et al.  Selective responding by recently separated juvenile rhesus monkeys to the calls of their mothers. , 1976, Developmental psychobiology.

[150]  Preston Somers Dialects in southern Rocky Mountain pikas, Ochotona princeps (Lagomorpha) , 1973 .

[151]  C. Snowdon,et al.  The vocalizations of pygmy marmosets (Cebuella pygmaea) , 1975, Animal Behaviour.

[152]  R. B. Payne,et al.  Social influences on vocal development: Field observations, experimental design, and the time and place of learning bird songs , 1997 .

[153]  R. Wrangham,et al.  Acoustic analysis of wild chimpanzee pant hoots: Do Kibale Forest chimpanzees have an acoustically distinct food arrival pant hoot? , 1993, American journal of primatology.

[154]  Poul Hansen Vocal learning: its role in adapting sound structures to long-distance propagation, and a hypothesis on its evolution , 1979, Animal Behaviour.

[155]  G. McCracken,et al.  Social organization and kinship in the polygynous bat Phyllostomus hastatus , 1981, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[156]  R. Zann Structural Variation in the Zebra Finch Distance Call , 1984 .

[157]  M. Feldman,et al.  Gene-culture coevolutionary theory. , 1996, Trends in ecology & evolution.

[158]  Dorothy L. Mammen,et al.  Individual differences and within-flock convergence in chickadee calls , 1981, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[159]  J. M. Trainer,et al.  Singing organization during aggressive interactions among male Yellow-rumped Caciques , 1988 .

[160]  W. Brockelman,et al.  Inheritance of stereotyped gibbon calls , 1984, Nature.

[161]  R. Dooling,et al.  Vocal development in budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus): contact calls. , 1997, Journal of comparative psychology.

[162]  R. B. Payne,et al.  Biological and cultural success of song memes in indigo buntings , 1988 .

[163]  K. Gibson,et al.  Mammalian social learning : comparative and ecological perspectives , 1999 .

[164]  B. McCowan,et al.  Spontaneous vocal mimicry and production by bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus): evidence for vocal learning. , 1993, Journal of comparative psychology.

[165]  P. Slater,et al.  Context-specific use suggests that bottlenose dolphin signature whistles are cohesion calls , 1998, Animal Behaviour.

[166]  E. Price The benefits of helpers: Effects of group and litter size on infant care in tamarins (Saguinus oedipus) , 1992, American journal of primatology.

[167]  William E. Evans,et al.  Whistles of bottlenose dolphins: Comparisons among populations , 1995 .

[168]  P. Arctander,et al.  Distribution of mtDNA haplotypes in North-Atlantic humpback whales: The influence of behavior on population structure , 1995 .

[169]  M. Konishi The role of auditory feedback in the control of vocalization in the white-crowned sparrow. , 1965, Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie.

[170]  P. Marler,et al.  Overproduction in song development: an evolutionary correlate with migration , 1996, Animal Behaviour.

[171]  E. Zimmermann,et al.  Acoustically Dimorphic Advertisement Calls Separate Morphologically and Genetically Homogenous Populations of the Grey Mouse Lemur (Microcebus murinus) , 1998, Folia Primatologica.

[172]  Karl-Heinz Esser,et al.  Vocal Dialects in the Lesser Spear-Nosed Bat Phyllostomus discolor , 1998, Naturwissenschaften.

[173]  W. Kinzey New world primates : ecology, evolution, and behavior , 1997 .

[174]  E. Wyndham Diurnal Cycle, Behaviour and Social Organization of the Budgerigar Melopsittacus Undulatus , 1980 .

[175]  M J Owren,et al.  Vocalizations of rhesus (Macaca mulatta) and Japanese (M. fuscata) macaques cross-fostered between species show evidence of only limited modification. , 1993, Developmental psychobiology.

[176]  C. Snowdon,et al.  Social influences on vocal development: Frontmatter , 1997 .

[177]  J. Wiens Bird Problems in Agriculture E. N. Wright I. R. Inglis C. J. Feare , 1982 .

[178]  W. Kinzey,et al.  The ecological role of the callitrichidae: a review. , 1984, American journal of physical anthropology.

[179]  N. Masataka The response of red-chested moustached tamarins to long calls from their natal and alien populations , 1988, Animal Behaviour.

[180]  R. Payne,et al.  Communication and Behavior of Whales. , 1986 .

[181]  B. Brockway Ethological Studies of the Budgerigar: Reproductive Behavior , 1964 .

[182]  H. Whitehead,et al.  Social organization of female sperm whales and their offspring: constant companions and casual acquaintances , 1991, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[183]  D. Tinkle,et al.  Natural Selection and Social Behavior: Recent Research and New Theory , 1981 .

[184]  L. Herman,et al.  Sex differences in site fidelity and migration of humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae) to the Hawaiian Islands , 1997 .

[185]  H. Schnitzler [Compensation of Doppler effects in horseshoe bats]. , 1967, Die Naturwissenschaften.

[186]  R. Haven Wiley,et al.  5 – Adaptations for Acoustic Communication in Birds: Sound Transmission and Signal Detection , 1982 .

[187]  H. Whitehead,et al.  Coda communication by sperm whales (Physeter macrocephalus) off the Galápagos Islands , 1993 .

[188]  W. M. Masters,et al.  Sonar signals of big brown bats, Eptesicus fuscus, contain information about individual identity, age and family affiliation , 1995, Animal Behaviour.

[189]  Stephen M. Kosslyn,et al.  Connectionism: There's something to it , 1985, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[190]  J. W. Boughman,et al.  Greater spear-nosed bats give group-distinctive calls , 1997, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[191]  Peter L. Tyack,et al.  DEVELOPMENT AND SOCIAL FUNCTIONS OF SIGNATURE WHISTLES IN BOTTLENOSE DOLPHINS TURSIOPS TRUNCATUS , 1997 .

[192]  Donald E. Kroodsma,et al.  The Function(s) of Bird Song , 1991 .

[193]  P L Tyack,et al.  A quantitative measure of similarity for tursiops truncatus signature whistles. , 1993, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[194]  Timothy F. Wright,et al.  Regional dialects in the contact call of a parrot , 1996, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[195]  J. Cheverud,et al.  Genetic differences among subspecies of the saddle‐back tamarin (Saguinus fuscicollis):evidence from hybrids , 1993, American journal of primatology.

[196]  Keith Bailey,et al.  The structure and variation of the separation call of the bobwhite quail (Colinus virginianus, Odontophorinae) , 1978, Animal Behaviour.

[197]  H. Schnitzler,et al.  Plasticity in echolocation signals of European pipistrelle bats in search flight: implications for habitat use and prey detection , 1993, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[198]  E. Gould Experimental studies of the ontogeny of ultrasonic vocalizations in bats. , 1975, Developmental psychobiology.

[199]  K. Esser,et al.  Detection of frequency modulation in the FM-bat Phyllostomus discolor , 1996, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[200]  G. Ellis,et al.  INTERCHANGE AND ISOLATION OF HUMPBACK WHALES OFF CALIFORNIA AND OTHER NORTH PACIFIC FEEDING GROUNDS , 1996 .

[201]  D. Lewis,et al.  Variations in underwater vocalizations of Weddell seals (Leptonychotes weddelli) at the Vestfold Hills as a measure of breeding population discreteness , 1988, Hydrobiologia.

[202]  Holger Preuschoft The Lesser Apes: Evolutionary and Behavioural Biology , 1985 .

[203]  Dialects in the alarm calls of prairie dogs , 1980, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[204]  R. B. Goldstein,et al.  Geographic Variation in the "Hoy" Call of the Bobwhite , 1978 .

[205]  E. Morton Ecological Sources of Selection on Avian Sounds , 1975, The American Naturalist.

[206]  Devra G. Kleiman,et al.  Advances in the study of mammalian behavior , 1983 .

[207]  N. Masataka,et al.  Development of vocal recognition of mothers in infant Japanese macaques. , 1985, Developmental psychobiology.

[208]  R. Seyfarth,et al.  Vocal development in vervet monkeys , 1986, Animal Behaviour.

[209]  J. Mann,et al.  Patterns of Female Attractiveness in Indian Ocean Bottlenose Dolphins , 1996 .

[210]  N. Øien,et al.  Populations genetic analysis of nuclear and mitochondrial loci in skin biopsies collected from central and northeastern North Atlantic humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae): population identity and migratory destinations , 1996, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[211]  R. Payne,et al.  Songs of Humpback Whales , 1971, Science.

[212]  Sources of phenotypic variation in the separation call of Northern Bobwhite (Colinus virginianus) , 1987 .

[213]  Randall S. Wells,et al.  Signature whistles of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus: stability and mother-offspring comparisons , 1990, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[214]  G. Wilkinson,et al.  Greater spear-nosed bats discriminate group mates by vocalizations , 1998, Animal Behaviour.

[215]  C. Snowdon,et al.  Troop‐specific responses to long calls of isolated tamarins (Saguinus mystax) , 1985, American journal of primatology.

[216]  Family- and sex-specific vocal traditions in a cooperatively breeding songbird , 1998 .

[217]  S. Nowicki,et al.  Flock-specific recognition of chickadee calls , 1983, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[218]  J T Marshall,et al.  Gibbons and Their Territorial Songs , 1976, Science.

[219]  H. Burton,et al.  Annual and diurnal variations in the underwater vocalizations of Weddell seals , 2004, Polar Biology.

[220]  K. Esser,et al.  Discrimination of sinusoidally frequency-modulated sound signals mimicking species-specific communication calls in the FM-bat Phyllostomus discolor , 1997, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[221]  John A. Wiens,et al.  SONG PATTERN VARIATION IN THE SAGE SPARROW (AMPHISPIZA BELLI): DIALECTS OR EPIPHENOMENA? , 1982 .

[222]  H. Whitehead,et al.  Sperm whale social units : variation and change , 1998 .

[223]  Gareth Jones,et al.  The development of vocalizations in Pipistrellus pipistrellus (Chiroptera: Vespertilionidae) during post-natal growth and the maintenance of individual vocal signatures , 1991 .

[224]  Robert M. Seyfarth,et al.  'Food' Calls Produced By Adult Female Rhesus (Macaca Mulatta) and Japanese (M. Fuscata) Macaques, Their Normally-Raised Offspring, and Offspring Cross-Fostered Between Species , 1992 .

[225]  H. D. Hoese Dolphin feeding out of water in a salt marsh. , 1971, Journal of mammalogy.

[226]  R. Zann Song and call learning in wild zebra finches in south-east Australia , 1990, Animal Behaviour.

[227]  P. Tyack,et al.  Differential response of humpback whales, Megaptera novaeangliae, to playback of song or social sounds , 1983, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[228]  A. Grinnell,et al.  Echolocation, development, and vocal communication in the lesser bulldog bat, Noctilio albiventris , 1983, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[229]  E. H. Haimoff,et al.  Loud Calls of the Gibbon (Hylobates Lar): Repertoire, Organisation and Context , 1984 .

[230]  A. M. Elowson,et al.  Ontogeny of trill and J-call vocalizations in the pygmy marmoset, Cebuella pygmaea , 1992, Animal Behaviour.

[231]  Randall S. Wells,et al.  The Social Structure of Free-Ranging Bottlenose Dolphins , 1987 .

[232]  Eugene S. Morton,et al.  6 – Grading, Discreteness, Redundancy, and Motivation-Structural Rules , 1982 .

[233]  C. Moss Ontogeny of Vocal Signals in the Big Brown Bat, Eptesicus Fuscus , 1988 .

[234]  P. Corkeron,et al.  Microsatellite genetic distances between oceanic populations of the humpback whale (Megaptera novaeangliae). , 1997, Molecular biology and evolution.

[235]  Tempo and mode of hummingbird evolution , 1998 .

[236]  H. Gouzoules,et al.  Design features and developmental modification of pigtail macaque, Macaca nemestrina, agonistic screams , 1989, Animal Behaviour.

[237]  Charles T. Snowdon,et al.  Pygmy marmosets, Cebuella pygmaea, modify vocal structure in response to changed social environment , 1994, Animal Behaviour.

[238]  G. Ehret Development of sound communication in mammals , 1980 .

[239]  R. Dooling,et al.  Effects of deafening on the calls and warble song of adult budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus). , 1999, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[240]  Peter L. Tyack,et al.  Social influences on vocal development: Vocal learning in cetaceans , 1997 .

[241]  H. Gouzoules,et al.  Recruitment screams of pigtail monkeys (Macaca nemestrina) : ontogenetic perspectives , 1995 .

[242]  P. Palsbøll,et al.  Molecular analysis of paternity shows promiscuous mating in female humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae, Borowski) , 1997, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[243]  M. Weinrich Stable social associations among humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae) in the southern Gulf of Maine , 1991 .

[244]  Charles T. Snowdon,et al.  Pygmy Marmosets modify call structure when paired , 1999 .

[245]  K. Esser,et al.  Hearing in the FM-bat Phyllostomus discolor: a behavioral audiogram , 1996, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[246]  Sandra L. L. Gaunt,et al.  Song Learning as Evidenced from Song Sharing in Two Hummingbird Species (Colibri coruscans and C. thalassinus) , 1994 .

[247]  S. Katona,et al.  Population composition of humpback whales, Megaptera novaeangliae, on Silver Bank, 1984 , 1989 .

[248]  Matrilineal signatures in the recruitment screams of pigtail macaques, Macaca nemestrina , 1990 .