Human Neuroscience

Hemispatial neglect is a debilitating disorder marked by a constellation of spatial and non-spatial attention deficits. Patients’ alertness deficits have shown to interact with lateralized attention processes and correspondingly, improving tonic/general alertness as well as phasic/moment-to-moment alertness has shown to ameliorate spatial bias. However, improvements are often short-lived and inconsistent across tasks and patients. In an attempt to more effectively activate alertness mechanisms by exercising both tonic and phasic alertness, we employed a novel version of a continuous performance task (tonic and phasic alertness training, TAPAT). Using a between-subjects longitudinal design and employing sensitive outcome measures of spatial and non-spatial attention, we compared the effects of 9 days of TAPAT (36 min/day) in a group of patients with chronic neglect (N = 12) with a control group of chronic neglect patients (N = 12) who simply waited during the same training period. Compared to the control group, the group trained on TAPAT significantly improved on both spatial and non-spatial measures of attention with many patients failing to exhibit a lateralized attention bias at the end of training. TAPAT was effective for patients with a range of behavioral profiles and lesions, suggesting that its effectiveness may rely on distributed or lower-level attention mechanisms that are largely intact in patients with neglect. In a follow-up experiment, to determine if TAPAT is more effective in improving spatial attention than an active treatment that directly trains spatial attention, we trained three chronic neglect patients on both TAPAT and search training. In all three patients, TAPAT training was more effective in improving spatial attention than search training suggesting that, in chronic neglect, training alertness is a more effective treatment approach than directly training spatial attention.

[1]  K. Heilman,et al.  Hypoarousal in patients with the neglect syndrome and emotional indifference , 1978, Neurology.

[2]  Stewart H Mostofsky,et al.  Interstimulus jitter facilitates response control in children with ADHD , 2009, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[3]  Ian H. Robertson,et al.  Personal Versus Extrapersonal Neglect: A Group Study of their Dissociation Using a Reliable Clinical Test , 1997, Cortex.

[4]  Michael Esterman,et al.  Visual hemispatial neglect, re-assessed , 2008, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[5]  Ivan Toni,et al.  Task instructions influence the cognitive strategies involved in line bisection judgements: evidence from modulated neural mechanisms revealed by fMRI , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[6]  Kenneth F Schulz,et al.  Generation of allocation sequences in randomised trials: chance, not choice , 2002, The Lancet.

[7]  A. Milner,et al.  A Revised Method for Analysing Neglect using the Landmark Task , 2004, Cortex.

[8]  W. Young,et al.  Reemergence of Stroke Deficits With Midazolam Challenge , 2002, Stroke.

[9]  M D'Esposito,et al.  Assessment of neglect reveals dissociable behavioral but not neuroanatomical subtypes , 1996, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[10]  A. Yamadori,et al.  Dissociation of body-centered and stimulus-centered representations in unilateral neglect , 2001, Neurology.

[11]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Improved picture naming in chronic aphasia after TMS to part of right Broca’s area: An open-protocol study , 2005, Brain and Language.

[12]  T. Salthouse Aging and measures of processing speed , 2000, Biological Psychology.

[13]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Decision making, the P3, and the locus coeruleus-norepinephrine system. , 2005, Psychological bulletin.

[14]  Jonathan Smallwood,et al.  Subjective experience and the attentional lapse: Task engagement and disengagement during sustained attention , 2004, Consciousness and Cognition.

[15]  C Kaernbach,et al.  A single-interval adjustment-matrix (SIAM) procedure for unbiased adaptive testing. , 1990, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[16]  M. Posner Measuring Alertness , 2008, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[17]  J. Mattingley,et al.  Phasic alerting of neglect patients overcomes their spatial deficit in visual awareness , 1998, Nature.

[18]  M. Bellgrove,et al.  Attenuation of spatial attentional asymmetries with poor sustained attention , 2004, Neuroreport.

[19]  Rainer Goebel,et al.  Neural correlates of conscious perception in the attentional blink , 2005, NeuroImage.

[20]  I. Robertson,et al.  `Oops!': Performance correlates of everyday attentional failures in traumatic brain injured and normal subjects , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[21]  M. Husain,et al.  Role of right posterior parietal cortex in maintaining attention to spatial locations over time , 2009, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[22]  Bogdan Draganski,et al.  Training-induced structural changes in the adult human brain , 2008, Behavioural Brain Research.

[23]  G. R. Fink,et al.  Impact of alertness training on spatial neglect: A behavioural and fMRI study , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[24]  Lynn C. Robertson,et al.  Cross-modal Interactions in Time and Space: Auditory Influence on Visual Attention in Hemispatial Neglect , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[25]  Kerstin Tham,et al.  Sustained attention and awareness of disability in chronic neglect , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[26]  H. Ring,et al.  Functional disability and rehabilitation outcome in right hemisphere damaged patients with and without unilateral spatial neglect. , 1999, Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation.

[27]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Neural basis and recovery of spatial attention deficits in spatial neglect , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[28]  Hillary S. Schaefer,et al.  Neural correlates of attentional expertise in long-term meditation practitioners , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[29]  L. Pizzamiglio,et al.  Development of a rehabilitative program for unilateral neglect. , 2006, Restorative neurology and neuroscience.

[30]  A. Nobre,et al.  The noradrenergic alpha2 agonist clonidine modulates behavioural and neuroanatomical correlates of human attentional orienting and alerting. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[31]  I. Nimmo-Smith,et al.  Sustained attention training for unilateral neglect: theoretical and rehabilitation implications. , 1995, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[32]  I. Radermacher,et al.  Functional anatomy of intrinsic alertness: evidencefor a fronto-parietal-thalamic-brainstem network in theright hemisphere , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[33]  K L Shapiro,et al.  Temporary suppression of visual processing in an RSVP task: an attentional blink? . , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[34]  F. Wilcoxon Individual Comparisons by Ranking Methods , 1945 .

[35]  Tom Manly,et al.  Auditory sustained attention is a marker of unilateral spatial neglect , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[36]  Yves Rossetti,et al.  Visuo-spatial neglect: A systematic review of current interventions and their effectiveness , 2006, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[37]  K. Willmes,et al.  On the Functional Neuroanatomy of Intrinsic and Phasic Alertness , 2001, NeuroImage.

[38]  S. Sara,et al.  Network reset: a simplified overarching theory of locus coeruleus noradrenaline function , 2005, Trends in Neurosciences.

[39]  C. Dodds,et al.  Rightward shift in spatial awareness with declining alertness , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[40]  Karsten Specht,et al.  Functional reorganisation in patients with right hemisphere stroke after training of alertness: a longitudinal PET and fMRI study in eight cases , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[41]  Karen M. Arnell,et al.  Attention to visual pattern information produces the attentional blink in rapid serial visual presentation. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[42]  J Whyte,et al.  Hemispatial neglect , 2004, Neurology.

[43]  Ian H. Robertson,et al.  Do We Need the “Lateral” in Unilateral Neglect? Spatially Nonselective Attention Deficits in Unilateral Neglect and Their Implications for Rehabilitation , 2001, NeuroImage.

[44]  A. Young,et al.  Neglect and visual recognition. , 1992, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[45]  D. Perani,et al.  Unilateral neglect: Personal and extra-personal , 1986, Neuropsychologia.

[46]  A. Milner,et al.  An Investigation of Hemispatial Neglect Using the Landmark Task , 1995, Brain and Cognition.

[47]  Sergio P. Rigonatti,et al.  Repeated sessions of noninvasive brain DC stimulation is associated with motor function improvement in stroke patients. , 2007, Restorative neurology and neuroscience.

[48]  Chris Rorden,et al.  Non-spatially lateralized mechanisms in hemispatial neglect , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[49]  Gereon R Fink,et al.  Cerebral correlates of alerting, orienting and reorienting of visuospatial attention: an event-related fMRI study , 2004, NeuroImage.

[50]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  An integrative theory of locus coeruleus-norepinephrine function: adaptive gain and optimal performance. , 2005, Annual review of neuroscience.

[51]  J. Driver,et al.  Visual Neglect , 1998 .

[52]  A. Kingstone,et al.  The effect of hemispatial neglect on the perception of centre , 2004, Brain and Cognition.

[53]  J. Marshall,et al.  Left neglect for near but not far space in man , 1991, Nature.

[54]  T. Robbins,et al.  Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[55]  C. Kennard,et al.  Abnormal temporal dynamics of visual attention in spatial neglect patients , 1997, Nature.

[56]  E. Bisiach,et al.  1 Hz repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of the unaffected hemisphere ameliorates contralesional visuospatial neglect in humans , 2003, Neuroscience Letters.

[57]  Yi-Yuan Tang,et al.  Attention training and attention state training , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[58]  J. Adair,et al.  Spatial Neglect: Clinical and Neuroscience Review , 2008, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.