Different spatial representations guide eye and hand movements.

Our visual system allows us to localize objects in the world and plan motor actions toward them. We have recently shown that the localization of moving objects differs between perception and saccadic eye movements (Lisi & Cavanagh, 2015), suggesting different localization mechanisms for perception and action. This finding, however, could reflect a unique feature of the saccade system rather than a general dissociation between perception and action. To disentangle these hypotheses, we compared object localization between saccades and hand movements. We flashed brief targets on top of double-drift stimuli (moving Gabors with the internal pattern drifting orthogonally to their displacement, inducing large distortions in perceived location and direction) and asked participants to point or make saccades to them. We found a surprising difference between the two types of movements: Although saccades targeted the physical location of the flashes, pointing movements were strongly biased toward the perceived location (about 63% of the perceptual illusion). The same bias was found when pointing movements were made in open-loop conditions (without vision of the hand). These results indicate that dissociations are present between different types of actions (not only between action and perception) and that visual processing for saccadic eye movements differs from that for other actions. Because the position bias in the double-drift stimulus depends on a persisting influence of past sensory signals, we suggest that spatial maps for saccades might reflect only recent, short-lived signals, and the spatial representations supporting conscious perception and hand movements integrate visual input over longer temporal intervals.

[1]  Patrick Cavanagh,et al.  The flash grab effect , 2012, Vision Research.

[2]  Volker H. Franz,et al.  The functional subdivision of the visual brain: Is there a real illusion effect on action? A multi-lab replication study , 2016, Cortex.

[3]  Harold E. Bedell,et al.  Spatial and temporal properties of the illusory motion-induced position shift for drifting stimuli , 2007, Vision Research.

[4]  H. Gomi Implicit online corrections of reaching movements , 2008, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[5]  H. Haladjian,et al.  On the evolution of conscious attention , 2014, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[6]  Patrick Cavanagh,et al.  Dissociation between the Perceptual and Saccadic Localization of Moving Objects , 2015, Current Biology.

[7]  M. Goodale,et al.  Two visual systems re-viewed , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[8]  Lawrence H Snyder,et al.  Idiosyncratic and systematic aspects of spatial representations in the macaque parietal cortex , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[9]  P.-J. Hsieh,et al.  The infinite regress illusion reveals faulty integration of local and global motion signals , 2006, Vision Research.

[10]  Robert L. Whitwell,et al.  Real and illusory issues in the illusion debate (Why two things are sometimes better than one): Commentary on Kopiske et al. (2016) , 2017, Cortex.

[11]  V. Franz,et al.  Action does not resist visual illusions , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[12]  Vincent P Ferrera,et al.  Neuronal responses to moving targets in monkey frontal eye fields. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  David C. Burr,et al.  Separate visual representations for perception and action revealed by saccadic eye movements , 2001, Current Biology.

[14]  T. Collins Trade-off between spatiotopy and saccadic plasticity. , 2014, Journal of vision.

[15]  Tirin Moore,et al.  Attention Governs Action in the Primate Frontal Eye Field , 2007, Neuron.

[16]  Ralf Engbert,et al.  Microsaccades are triggered by low retinal image slip. , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[17]  H. Haladjian,et al.  Consciousness, Attention, and Conscious Attention , 2015 .

[18]  Karl R. Gegenfurtner,et al.  Saccade Adaptation and Visual Uncertainty , 2016, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[19]  David C Knill,et al.  Visual Feedback Control of Hand Movements , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[20]  J. Saunders,et al.  Humans use continuous visual feedback from the hand to control fast reaching movements , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[21]  Pratik K. Mutha,et al.  The influence of visual target information on the online control of movements , 2015, Vision Research.

[22]  Laurent Goffart,et al.  Does the Brain Extrapolate the Position of a Transient Moving Target? , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[23]  L. Jakobson,et al.  A neurological dissociation between perceiving objects and grasping them , 1991, Nature.

[24]  David Whitney,et al.  Visual motion shifts saccade targets , 2014, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.

[25]  M. Goodale,et al.  Separate visual pathways for perception and action , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.

[26]  Chang-Bing Huang,et al.  Transitions between Central and Peripheral Vision Create Spatial/Temporal Distortions: A Hypothesis Concerning the Perceived Break of the Curveball , 2010, PloS one.

[27]  R. McIntosh,et al.  Do we have independent visual streams for perception and action? , 2010, Cognitive neuroscience.

[28]  David C Knill,et al.  Decoupling eye and hand movement control: visual short-term memory influences reach planning more than saccade planning. , 2012, Journal of vision.

[29]  P Fattori,et al.  Multiple representation of reaching space in the medial posterior parietal area V6A. , 2015, Cerebral cortex.

[30]  Iain D Gilchrist,et al.  The target velocity integration function for saccades. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[31]  R. Gellman,et al.  Motion processing for saccadic eye movements in humans , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[32]  F A Miles,et al.  Short latency ocular-following responses in man , 1990, Visual Neuroscience.

[33]  Nicola Bruno,et al.  When does action resist visual illusions? , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[34]  Marco Zorzi,et al.  Paying Attention through Eye Movements: A Computational Investigation of the Premotor Theory of Spatial Attention , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[35]  Shinsuke Shimojo,et al.  The Hand Sees Visual Periphery Better Than the Eye: Motor-Dependent Visual Motion Analyses , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[36]  Duje Tadin,et al.  Unifying account of visual motion and position perception , 2015, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[37]  Katherine M. Armstrong,et al.  Visuomotor Origins of Covert Spatial Attention , 2003, Neuron.

[38]  C. Tallon-Baudry,et al.  Neural Dissociation between Visual Awareness and Spatial Attention , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[39]  M. Goodale,et al.  Size-contrast illusions deceive the eye but not the hand , 1995, Current Biology.

[40]  Bruce Bridgeman,et al.  The induced Roelofs effect: two visual systems or the shift of a single reference frame? , 2004, Vision Research.

[41]  B. Bridgeman,et al.  Segregation of cognitive and motor aspects of visual function using induced motion , 1981, Perception & psychophysics.

[42]  A. Gorea,et al.  On the perceptual/motor dissociation: a review of concepts, theory, experimental paradigms and data interpretations. , 2010, Seeing and perceiving.

[43]  C. de’Sperati,et al.  Blind Saccades: An Asynchrony between Seeing and Looking , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[44]  Karl K. Kopiske,et al.  Do visual illusions affect grasping? Considerable progress in a scientific debate. A reply to Whitwell & Goodale, 2016 , 2017, Cortex.

[45]  David C. Knill,et al.  Integrating visual cues for motor control: A matter of time , 2005, Vision Research.