The Taste of Music

Zarlino, one of the most important music theorists of the XVI century, described the minor consonances as ‘sweet’ (dolci) and ‘soft’ (soavi) (Zarlino 1558/1983, in On the Modes New Haven, CT: Yale University Press, 1983). Hector Berlioz, in his Treatise on Modern Instrumentation and Orchestration (London: Novello, 1855), speaks about the ‘small acid-sweet voice’ of the oboe. In line with this tradition of describing musical concepts in terms of taste words, recent empirical studies have found reliable associations between taste perception and low-level sound and musical parameters, like pitch and phonetic features. Here we investigated whether taste words elicited consistent musical representations by asking trained musicians to improvise on the basis of the four canonical taste words: sweet, sour, bitter, and salty. Our results showed that, even in free improvisation, taste words elicited very reliable and consistent musical patterns: ‘bitter’ improvisations are low-pitched and legato (without interruption between notes), ‘salty’ improvisations are staccato (notes sharply detached from each other), ‘sour’ improvisations are high-pitched and dissonant, and ‘sweet’ improvisations are consonant, slow, and soft. Interestingly, projections of the improvisations of taste words to musical space (a vector space defined by relevant musical parameters) revealed that, in musical space, improvisations based on different taste words were nearly orthogonal or opposite. Decoding methods could classify binary choices of improvisations (ie identify the improvisation word from the melody) at performance of around 80%—well above chance. In a second experiment we investigated the mapping from perception of music to taste words. Fifty-seven non-musical experts listened to a fraction of the improvisations. We found that listeners classified with high performance the taste word which had elicited the improvisation. Our results, furthermore, show that associations of taste and music go beyond basic sensory attributes into the domain of semantics, and open a new venue of investigation to understand the origins of these consistent taste–musical patterns.

[1]  V. Ramachandran,et al.  Hearing colors, tasting shapes. , 2005, Scientific American.

[2]  R. Plomp,et al.  Tonal consonance and critical bandwidth. , 1965, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[3]  Edward M. Hubbard,et al.  Neurocognitive Mechanisms of Synesthesia , 2005, Neuron.

[4]  Lutz Jäncke,et al.  The multiple synaesthete E.S. — Neuroanatomical basis of interval-taste and tone-colour synaesthesia , 2008, NeuroImage.

[5]  S. Kirby,et al.  What Sound Does That Taste? Cross-Modal Mappings across Gustation and Audition , 2010, Perception.

[6]  D. Small,et al.  Odor/taste integration and the perception of flavor , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[7]  Anne Treisman,et al.  Natural cross-modal mappings between visual and auditory features. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[8]  P. Breslin,et al.  Mammalian taste perception , 2008, Current Biology.

[9]  R. Zatorre,et al.  Odor-induced changes in taste perception , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[10]  C. Spence,et al.  Multisensory perception: Beyond modularity and convergence , 2000, Current Biology.

[11]  Charles Spence,et al.  Implicit association between basic tastes and pitch , 2009, Neuroscience Letters.

[12]  Jamie Ward,et al.  Sound-Colour Synaesthesia: to What Extent Does it Use Cross-Modal Mechanisms Common to us All? , 2006, Cortex.

[13]  H. L. Jacobs,et al.  Sugar Sweetness and Pleasantness: Evidence for Different Psychological Laws , 1974, Science.

[14]  R. Zatorre,et al.  Music, the food of neuroscience? , 2005, Nature.

[15]  Giovanni De Poli,et al.  An Abstract Control Space for Communication of Sensory Expressive Intentions in Music Performance , 2003 .

[16]  Cesare Parise,et al.  Synesthetic congruency modulates the temporal ventriloquism effect , 2008, Neuroscience Letters.

[17]  L. Marks,et al.  Cross-Modal Interaction between Vision and Touch: The Role of Synesthetic Correspondence , 2000, Perception.

[18]  R. Stevenson,et al.  Olfactory-induced synesthesias: a review and model. , 2007, Psychological bulletin.

[19]  A. Friederici,et al.  Music, language and meaning: brain signatures of semantic processing , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[20]  C. Spence,et al.  As bitter as a trombone: Synesthetic correspondences in nonsynesthetes between tastes/flavors and musical notes , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[21]  Charles Spence,et al.  Cross-modal associations between odors and colors. , 2006, Chemical senses.

[22]  Hector Berlioz A treatise upon modern instrumentation and orchestration , 1858 .

[23]  Giovanni De Poli,et al.  Communicating expressive intentions with a single piano note , 2006 .

[24]  J. Russell A circumplex model of affect. , 1980 .

[25]  C. Spence,et al.  The multisensory perception of flavor , 2008, Consciousness and Cognition.

[26]  Charles Spence,et al.  A Sweet Sound? Food Names Reveal Implicit Associations between Taste and Pitch , 2010, Perception.

[27]  Lutz Jäncke,et al.  Synaesthesia: When coloured sounds taste sweet , 2005, Nature.