Rostrolateral Prefrontal Cortex Involvement in Relational Integration during Reasoning

Patient and neuroimaging studies indicate that complex reasoning tasks are associated with the prefrontal cortex (PFC). In this study, we tested the hypothesis that the process of relational integration, or considering multiple relations simultaneously, is a component process of complex reasoning that selectively recruits PFC. We used fMRI to examine brain activation during 0-relational, 1-relational, and 2-relational problems adapted from the Raven's Progressive Matrices and hypothesized that PFC would be preferentially recruited by the 2-relational problem type. Event-related responses were modeled by convolving a canonical hemodynamic response function with the response time (RT) associated with each trial. The results across different analyses revealed the same pattern: PFC activation was specific to the comparison between 2- and 1-relational problems and was not observed in the comparison between 1- and 0-relational problems. Furthermore, the process of relational integration was specifically associated with bilateral rostrolateral PFC (RLPFC; lateral area 10) and right dorsolateral PFC (areas 9 and 46). Left RLPFC showed the greatest specificity by remaining preferentially recruited during 2-relational problems even after comparisons were restricted to trials matched for RT and accuracy. The link between RLPFC and the process of relational integration may be due to the associated process of manipulating self-generated information, a process that may characterize RLPFC function.

[1]  J. Raven STANDARDIZATION OF PROGRESSIVE MATRICES, 1938 , 1941 .

[2]  B. Milner Effects of Different Brain Lesions on Card Sorting: The Role of the Frontal Lobes , 1963 .

[3]  J. M. Warren,et al.  THE FRONTAL GRANULAR CORTEX AND BEHAVIOR , 1964 .

[4]  H. E. Rosvold,et al.  Behavioral effects of selective ablation of the caudate nucleus. , 1967, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[5]  R. D. Hunt,et al.  Huntington's dementia. Clinical and neuropsychological features. , 1978, Archives of general psychiatry.

[6]  A. Luria Higher Cortical Functions in Man , 1980, Springer US.

[7]  G. Halford,et al.  A category theory approach to cognitive development , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.

[8]  A. Cools,et al.  Evidence for a role of the caudate nucleus in the sequential organization of behaviour , 1982, Behavioural Brain Research.

[9]  G. Halford Can young children integrate premises in transitivity and serial order tasks? , 1984, Cognitive Psychology.

[10]  G. E. Alexander,et al.  Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. , 1986, Annual review of neuroscience.

[11]  J. Talairach,et al.  Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain: 3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging , 1988 .

[12]  Mario F. Mendez,et al.  Neurobehavioral changes associated with caudate lesions , 1989, Neurology.

[13]  M A Just,et al.  From the SelectedWorks of Marcel Adam Just 1990 What one intelligence test measures : A theoretical account of the processing in the Raven Progressive Matrices Test , 2016 .

[14]  T. Robbins,et al.  Planning and spatial working memory following frontal lobe lesions in man , 1990, Neuropsychologia.

[15]  Tim Shallice,et al.  HIGHER-ORDER COGNITIVE IMPAIRMENTS AND FRONTAL-LOBE LESIONS IN MAN , 1991 .

[16]  A. Benton,et al.  Frontal Lobe Function and Dysfunction , 1991 .

[17]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Assessing the significance of focal activations using their spatial extent , 1994, Human brain mapping.

[18]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: A general linear approach , 1994 .

[19]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Brain regions associated with acquisition and retrieval of verbal episodic memory , 1994, Nature.

[20]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  The neural basis of the central executive system of working memory , 1995, Nature.

[21]  G H Glover,et al.  Motion Artifacts in fMRI: Comparison of 2DFT with PR and Spiral Scan Methods , 1995, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[22]  J. Cohen,et al.  Spiral K‐space MR imaging of cortical activation , 1995, Journal of magnetic resonance imaging : JMRI.

[23]  Richard Coppola,et al.  Physiological activation of a cortical network during performance of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: A positron emission tomography study , 1995, Neuropsychologia.

[24]  E. Bizzi,et al.  The Cognitive Neurosciences , 1996 .

[25]  R. Dolan,et al.  Neural systems engaged by planning: a PET study of the Tower of London task , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[26]  D. Heeger,et al.  Linear Systems Analysis of Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging in Human V1 , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Planning and Spatial Working Memory: a Positron Emission Tomography Study in Humans , 1996, The European journal of neuroscience.

[28]  D Bonner,et al.  Acute Frontal Lobe Syndrome and Dyscontrol Associated with Bilateral Caudate Nucleus Infarctions , 1996, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[29]  J. Desmond,et al.  Neural Substrates of Fluid Reasoning: An fMRI Study of Neocortical Activation during Performance of the Raven's Progressive Matrices Test , 1997, Cognitive Psychology.

[30]  J. A. Detre,et al.  The Effect of Pacing of Experimental Stimuli on Observed Functional MRI Activity , 1997, NeuroImage.

[31]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  BrainWeb: Online Interface to a 3D MRI Simulated Brain Database , 1997 .

[32]  J. Binder,et al.  Functional MRI evidence for subcortical participation in conceptual reasoning skills , 1997, Neuroreport.

[33]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Event‐related f MRI , 1997, Human brain mapping.

[34]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Temporal dynamics of brain activation during a working memory task , 1997, Nature.

[35]  S. Kapur,et al.  The seats of reason? An imaging study of deductive and inductive reasoning , 1997, Neuroreport.

[36]  Mark S. Cohen,et al.  Parametric Analysis of fMRI Data Using Linear Systems Methods , 1997, NeuroImage.

[37]  長濱康弘,et al.  Cerebral activation during performance of a Card Sorting Test(カード分類検査の実行中に観察される大脳の賦活部位) , 1997 .

[38]  T. Goldberg,et al.  Uncoupling Cognitive Workload and Prefrontal Cortical Physiology: A PET rCBF Study , 1998, NeuroImage.

[39]  G. Glover,et al.  Self‐navigated spiral fMRI: Interleaved versus single‐shot , 1998, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[40]  S. Phillips,et al.  Processing capacity defined by relational complexity: implications for comparative, developmental, and cognitive psychology. , 1998, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[41]  M. Raichle,et al.  The neural correlates of consciousness: an analysis of cognitive skill learning. , 1998, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[42]  R. Turner,et al.  Event-Related fMRI: Characterizing Differential Responses , 1998, NeuroImage.

[43]  Vinod Goel,et al.  Neuroanatomical Correlates of Human Reasoning , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[44]  A. Alavi,et al.  Frontotemporal cerebral blood flow change during executive and declarative memory tasks in schizophrenia: a positron emission tomography study. , 1998, Neuropsychology.

[45]  D. Osherson,et al.  Distinct brain loci in deductive versus probabilistic reasoning , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[46]  D. Noll,et al.  Nonlinear Aspects of the BOLD Response in Functional MRI , 1998, NeuroImage.

[47]  J. Ashburner,et al.  Nonlinear spatial normalization using basis functions , 1999, Human brain mapping.

[48]  G. Glover Deconvolution of Impulse Response in Event-Related BOLD fMRI1 , 1999, NeuroImage.

[49]  E. Koechlin,et al.  The role of the anterior prefrontal cortex in human cognition , 1999, Nature.

[50]  K. Holyoak,et al.  A System for Relational Reasoning in Human Prefrontal Cortex , 1999 .

[51]  A. Dagher,et al.  Mapping the network for planning: a correlational PET activation study with the Tower of London task. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[52]  R. Cabeza,et al.  Imaging Cognition II: An Empirical Review of 275 PET and fMRI Studies , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[53]  J. Gabrieli,et al.  The frontopolar cortex and human cognition: Evidence for a rostrocaudal hierarchical organization within the human prefrontal cortex , 2000, Psychobiology.

[54]  M. Honda,et al.  Toward Neuroanatomical Models of Analogy: A Positron Emission Tomography Study of Analogical Mapping , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.

[55]  R. Dolan,et al.  Dissociation of Mechanisms Underlying Syllogistic Reasoning , 2000, NeuroImage.

[56]  Vinod Goel,et al.  Anatomical Segregation of Component Processes in an Inductive Inference Task , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[57]  G L Shulman,et al.  INAUGURAL ARTICLE by a Recently Elected Academy Member:A default mode of brain function , 2001 .

[58]  J. Lancaster,et al.  Using the talairach atlas with the MNI template , 2001, NeuroImage.

[59]  J D Gabrieli,et al.  Neural substrates of mathematical reasoning: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study of neocortical activation during performance of the necessary arithmetic operations test. , 2001, Neuropsychology.

[60]  John D. E. Gabrieli,et al.  Rostrolateral prefrontal cortex involvement in evaluating self-generated information , 2001, NeuroImage.

[61]  James K. Kroger,et al.  Recruitment of anterior dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in human reasoning: a parametric study of relational complexity. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.