Estimating retest effects in longitudinal assessments of cognitive functioning in adults between 18 and 60 years of age.

Several analyses were conducted on data from samples of adults between 18 and 58 years of age who completed the same cognitive tests after an interval ranging from less than 1 week to 35 years. Because the retest interval varied across individuals, it was possible to determine the length of time needed before the gains associated with a retest decreased to 0 and to obtain simultaneous estimates of the magnitude of effects associated with increased age and a prior assessment. The results indicated that for adults within this age range, 7 or more years were needed before positive retest effects were no longer detectable. Age effects in longitudinal comparisons could be interpreted in terms of large positive effects associated with a prior assessment and negative effects associated with age that were comparable in magnitude to those observed in cross-sectional comparisons.

[1]  C B Dodrill,et al.  Test–retest bias, reliability, and regression equations for neuropsychological measures repeated over a 12–16-week period , 2001, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[2]  J. Mcardle,et al.  Comparative longitudinal structural analyses of the growth and decline of multiple intellectual abilities over the life span. , 2002, Developmental psychology.

[3]  T. Salthouse,et al.  Age-related effects on cognition between 20 and 50 years of age , 2004 .

[4]  T. Salthouse,et al.  Shared age-related influences on cognitive and noncognitive variables. , 1998, Psychology and aging.

[5]  J. Schneider,et al.  Individual differences in rates of change in cognitive abilities of older persons. , 2002, Psychology and aging.

[6]  N. Raz Aging of the brain and its impact on cognitive performance: Integration of structural and functional findings. , 2000 .

[7]  K. Schaie Internal Validity Threats in Studies of Adult Cognitive Development , 1988 .

[8]  I. Grant,et al.  Test–retest reliability and practice effects of Expanded Halstead–Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery , 1999, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[9]  A. Mackinnon,et al.  Age is no kinder to the better educated: absence of an association investigated using latent growth techniques in a community sample , 2001, Psychological Medicine.

[10]  Robert M. Yerkes,et al.  Psychological examining in the United States army. , 1921 .

[11]  H. E. Jones,et al.  The growth and decline of intelligence: a study of a homogeneous group between the ages of ten and sixty. , 1933 .

[12]  Suzanne E. Welcome,et al.  Mapping cortical change across the human life span , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[13]  H. Christensen,et al.  Is age kinder to the initially more able? A study of eminent scientists and academics , 1991, Psychological Medicine.

[14]  W. Snow,et al.  WAIS-R test-retest reliability in a normal elderly sample. , 1989, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[15]  T. Salthouse,et al.  What needs to be explained to account for age-related effects on multiple cognitive variables? , 2003, Psychology and aging.

[16]  M. O’Sullivan,et al.  Activate your online subscription , 2001, Neurology.

[17]  P. Satz,et al.  Effect of repeated administration of a neuropsychological battery in the elderly. , 1991, Journal of clinical psychology.

[18]  Timothy A. Salthouse,et al.  The Nature of the Influence of Speed on Adult Age Differences in Cognition. , 1994 .

[19]  W. Owens Is age kinder to the initially more able? , 1959, Journal of gerontology.

[20]  P. Vernon Heredity and Environment in the Growth and Decline of Intelligence. , 1975 .

[21]  M S Buchsbaum,et al.  Regional and global changes in cerebral diffusion with normal aging. , 2001, AJNR. American journal of neuroradiology.

[22]  Ulric Neisser,et al.  The rising curve: Long-term gains in IQ and related measures. , 1998 .

[23]  R. D. Tuddenham Soldier intelligence in World Wars I and II. , 1948, The American psychologist.

[24]  Paul B. Baltes,et al.  Intellectual Development in Adulthood: The Seattle Longitudinal Study , 1996 .

[25]  T Horinouchi,et al.  Development and aging of the cerebrum: assessment with proton MR spectroscopy. , 2001, AJNR. American journal of neuroradiology.

[26]  J. Becker,et al.  Alternate forms of the Auditory-Verbal Learning Test: issues of test comparability, longitudinal reliability, and moderating demographic variables. , 1995, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.

[27]  R. Dixon,et al.  Age-related cognitive deficits mediated by changes in the striatal dopamine system. , 2000, The American journal of psychiatry.

[28]  P. Diggle,et al.  Identifying and separating the effects of practice and of cognitive ageing during a large longitudinal study of elderly community residents , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[29]  Haruyasu Yamada,et al.  Normal aging in the central nervous system: quantitative MR diffusion-tensor analysis , 2002, Neurobiology of Aging.

[30]  F. Watson,et al.  Nine parallel versions of four memory tests: An assessment of form equivalence and the effects of practice on performance , 1994 .

[31]  J. Jolles,et al.  The relation between global and limbic brain volumes on MRI and cognitive performance in healthy individuals across the age range , 2000, Neurobiology of Aging.

[32]  E. L. Mortensen,et al.  A gender difference in the association between APOE genotype and age-related cognitive decline , 2001, Neurology.

[33]  Pamela K. Smith,et al.  Models of visuospatial and verbal memory across the adult life span. , 2002, Psychology and aging.

[34]  N. Volkow,et al.  Association between decline in brain dopamine activity with age and cognitive and motor impairment in healthy individuals. , 1998, The American journal of psychiatry.

[35]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A Voxel-Based Morphometric Study of Ageing in 465 Normal Adult Human Brains , 2001, NeuroImage.

[36]  Emilio Ferrer,et al.  Modeling age and retest processes in longitudinal studies of cognitive abilities. , 2004, Psychology and aging.

[37]  N. Volkow,et al.  Association between age-related decline in brain dopamine activity and impairment in frontal and cingulate metabolism. , 2000, The American journal of psychiatry.

[38]  Patrick Rabbitt,et al.  Test\re-test reliability of the CANTAB and ISPOCD neuropsychological batteries: theoretical and practical issues , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[39]  I. Deary,et al.  Is age kinder to the initially more able?: Differential ageing of a verbal ability in the , 1998 .

[40]  Nora D. Volkow,et al.  Dr. Volkow and Colleagues Reply , 1987 .

[41]  Lars Farde,et al.  Age-related dopamine D2/D3 receptor loss in extrastriatal regions of the human brain , 2000, Neurobiology of Aging.

[42]  G. Bartzokis,et al.  Age-related changes in frontal and temporal lobe volumes in men: a magnetic resonance imaging study. , 2001, Archives of general psychiatry.

[43]  F. Huppert,et al.  The health and lifestyle survey : seven years on , 1993 .

[44]  John J. McArdle,et al.  Expanding Test-Retest Designs to Include Developmental Time-Lag Components , 2004 .

[45]  T. Salthouse,et al.  Meta-analyses of age-cognition relations in adulthood: estimates of linear and nonlinear age effects and structural models. , 1997, Psychological bulletin.

[46]  R. Kikinis,et al.  White matter changes with normal aging , 1998, Neurology.

[47]  H. Buschke,et al.  Cross-sectional and longitudinal relationships among age, cognition, and processing speed. , 1999, Psychology and aging.

[48]  S J Czaja,et al.  Structural constraints on process explanations in cognitive aging. , 2000, Psychology and aging.

[49]  Timothy A. Salthouse,et al.  Speed Mediation of Adult Age Differences in Cognition , 2005 .

[50]  C. Brainerd,et al.  Cognitive development in adulthood : progress in cognitive development research , 1988 .

[51]  R. Benedict,et al.  Practice effects during repeated administrations of memory tests with and without alternate forms. , 1998, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.