Estimates of the Contribution of Single Neurons to Perception Depend on Timescale and Noise Correlation

The sensitivity of a population of neurons, and therefore the amount of sensory information available to an animal, is limited by the sensitivity of single neurons in the population and by noise correlation between neurons. For decades, therefore, neurophysiologists have devised increasingly clever and rigorous ways to measure these critical variables (Parker and Newsome, 1998). Previous studies examining the relationship between the responses of single middle temporal (MT) neurons and direction-discrimination performance uncovered an apparent paradox. Sensitivity measurements from single neurons suggested that small numbers of neurons may account for a monkey's psychophysical performance (Britten et al., 1992), but trial-to-trial variability in activity of single MT neurons are only weakly correlated with the monkey's behavior, suggesting that the monkey's decision must be based on the responses of many neurons (Britten et al., 1996). We suggest that the resolution to this paradox lies (1) in the long stimulus duration used in the original studies, which led to an overestimate of neural sensitivity relative to psychophysical sensitivity, and (2) mistaken assumptions (because no data were available) about the level of noise correlation in MT columns with opposite preferred directions. We therefore made new physiological and psychophysical measurements in a reaction time version of the direction-discrimination task that matches neural measurements to the actual decision time of the animals. These new data, considered together with our recent data on noise correlation in MT (Cohen and Newsome, 2008), provide a substantially improved account of psychometric performance in the direction-discrimination task.

[1]  D. M. Green,et al.  Signal detection theory and psychophysics , 1966 .

[2]  E. Evarts A technique for recording activity of subcortical neurons in moving animals. , 1968, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[3]  B. Richmond,et al.  Implantation of magnetic search coils for measurement of eye position: An improved method , 1980, Vision Research.

[4]  W. Newsome,et al.  Deficits in visual motion processing following ibotenic acid lesions of the middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[5]  D G Pelli,et al.  Probe tone thresholds in the auditory nerve measured by two-interval forced-choice procedures. , 1987, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[6]  W. Newsome,et al.  A selective impairment of motion perception following lesions of the middle temporal visual area (MT) , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[7]  K. H. Britten,et al.  Neuronal correlates of a perceptual decision , 1989, Nature.

[8]  William T. Newsome,et al.  Cortical microstimulation influences perceptual judgements of motion direction , 1990, Nature.

[9]  J. Movshon,et al.  The analysis of visual motion: a comparison of neuronal and psychophysical performance , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[10]  P. H. Schiller,et al.  The effects of V4 and middle temporal (MT) area lesions on visual performance in the rhesus monkey , 1993, Visual Neuroscience.

[11]  K. H. Britten,et al.  Responses of neurons in macaque MT to stochastic motion signals , 1993, Visual Neuroscience.

[12]  C D Salzman,et al.  Neural mechanisms for forming a perceptual decision. , 1994, Science.

[13]  P H Schiller,et al.  The effects of lateral geniculate nucleus, area V4, and middle temporal (MT) lesions on visually guided eye movements , 1994, Visual Neuroscience.

[14]  W. Newsome,et al.  Neuronal and psychophysical sensitivity to motion signals in extrastriate area MST of the macaque monkey , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[15]  Ehud Zohary,et al.  Correlated neuronal discharge rate and its implications for psychophysical performance , 1994, Nature.

[16]  G. Orban,et al.  Lesions of the Superior Temporal Cortical Motion Areas Impair Speed Discrimination in the Macaque Monkey , 1995, The European journal of neuroscience.

[17]  Wyeth Bair Analysis of temporal structure in spike trains of visual cortical area MT , 1996 .

[18]  J. Movshon,et al.  A computational analysis of the relationship between neuronal and behavioral responses to visual motion , 1996, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[19]  K. H. Britten,et al.  A relationship between behavioral choice and the visual responses of neurons in macaque MT , 1996, Visual Neuroscience.

[20]  A. Parker,et al.  Sense and the single neuron: probing the physiology of perception. , 1998, Annual review of neuroscience.

[21]  A. P. Georgopoulos,et al.  Variability and Correlated Noise in the Discharge of Neurons in Motor and Parietal Areas of the Primate Cortex , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[22]  G. DeAngelis,et al.  Cortical area MT and the perception of stereoscopic depth , 1998, Nature.

[23]  T. Pasternak,et al.  Transient and permanent deficits in motion perception after lesions of cortical areas MT and MST in the macaque monkey. , 1999, Cerebral cortex.

[24]  J. Donoghue,et al.  Neuronal Interactions Improve Cortical Population Coding of Movement Direction , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[25]  A. Parker,et al.  The Precision of Single Neuron Responses in Cortical Area V1 during Stereoscopic Depth Judgments , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  R. Romo,et al.  Neuronal correlates of sensory discrimination in the somatosensory cortex. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[27]  G H Recanzone,et al.  Correlation between the activity of single auditory cortical neurons and sound-localization behavior in the macaque monkey. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[28]  A. Parker,et al.  Perceptually Bistable Three-Dimensional Figures Evoke High Choice Probabilities in Cortical Area MT , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  W. Bair,et al.  Correlated Firing in Macaque Visual Area MT: Time Scales and Relationship to Behavior , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[30]  T. Pasternak,et al.  Microstimulation of cortical area MT affects performance on a visual working memory task. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[31]  William T Newsome,et al.  Middle Temporal Visual Area Microstimulation Influences Veridical Judgments of Motion Direction , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[32]  Xiao-Jing Wang,et al.  Probabilistic Decision Making by Slow Reverberation in Cortical Circuits , 2002, Neuron.

[33]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  Dynamics of neuronal responses in macaque MT and VIP during motion detection , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[34]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Response of Neurons in the Lateral Intraparietal Area during a Combined Visual Discrimination Reaction Time Task , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[35]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Microstimulation of visual cortex affects the speed of perceptual decisions , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[36]  G. DeAngelis,et al.  Contribution of Middle Temporal Area to Coarse Depth Discrimination: Comparison of Neuronal and Psychophysical Sensitivity , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[37]  Daeyeol Lee,et al.  Neural Noise and Movement-Related Codes in the Macaque Supplementary Motor Area , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[38]  M. Shadlen,et al.  A role for neural integrators in perceptual decision making. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[39]  A. Parker,et al.  Comparing perceptual signals of single V5/MT neurons in two binocular depth tasks. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[40]  G. DeAngelis,et al.  Contribution of Area MT to Stereoscopic Depth Perception Choice-Related Response Modulations Reflect Task Strategy , 2004, Neuron.

[41]  G. DeAngelis,et al.  Perceptual “Read-Out” of Conjoined Direction and Disparity Maps in Extrastriate Area MT , 2004, PLoS biology.

[42]  D. Bradley,et al.  Neural population code for fine perceptual decisions in area MT , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[43]  S. Treue,et al.  The response of neurons in areas V1 and MT of the alert rhesus monkey to moving random dot patterns , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[44]  Brian A Wandell,et al.  Cone signal interactions in direction-selective neurons in the middle temporal visual area (MT). , 2005, Journal of vision.

[45]  M. A. Smith,et al.  Stimulus Dependence of Neuronal Correlation in Primary Visual Cortex of the Macaque , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[46]  D. Levi,et al.  Receptive versus perceptive fields from the reverse-correlation viewpoint , 2006, Vision Research.

[47]  B. Cumming,et al.  Macaque V2 Neurons, But Not V1 Neurons, Show Choice-Related Activity , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[48]  W. Newsome,et al.  Local Field Potential in Cortical Area MT: Stimulus Tuning and Behavioral Correlations , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[49]  Timothy D. Hanks,et al.  Bounded Integration in Parietal Cortex Underlies Decisions Even When Viewing Duration Is Dictated by the Environment , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[50]  W. Newsome,et al.  Context-Dependent Changes in Functional Circuitry in Visual Area MT , 2008, Neuron.

[51]  M. A. Smith,et al.  Spatial and Temporal Scales of Neuronal Correlation in Primary Visual Cortex , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[52]  H. Komatsu,et al.  Relationship between color discrimination and neural responses in the inferior temporal cortex of the monkey. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[53]  Maik C. Stüttgen,et al.  Psychophysical and neurometric detection performance under stimulus uncertainty , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[54]  H. Barlow,et al.  Single Units and Sensation: A Neuron Doctrine for Perceptual Psychology? , 1972, Perception.

[55]  B. Cumming,et al.  Decision-related activity in sensory neurons reflects more than a neuron’s causal effect , 2009, Nature.