Behavioral/systems/cognitive Effective Connectivity Reveals Important Roles for Both the Hyperdirect (fronto-subthalamic) and the Indirect (fronto-striatal-pallidal) Fronto-basal Ganglia Pathways during Response Inhibition

Fronto-basal ganglia pathways play a crucial role in voluntary action control, including the ability to inhibit motor responses. Response inhibition might be mediated via a fast hyperdirect pathway connecting the right inferior frontal gyrus (rIFG) and the presupplementary motor area (preSMA) with the subthalamic nucleus or, alternatively, via the indirect pathway between the cortex and caudate. To test the relative contribution of these two pathways to inhibitory action control, we applied an innovative quantification method for effective brain connectivity. Functional magnetic resonance imaging data were collected from 20 human participants performing a Simon interference task with an occasional stop signal. A single right-lateralized model involving both the hyperdirect and indirect pathways best explained the pattern of brain activation on stop trials. Notably, the overall connection strength of this combined model was highest on successfully inhibited trials. Inspection of the relationship between behavior and connection values revealed that fast inhibitors showed increased connectivity between rIFG and right caudate (rCaudate), whereas slow inhibitors were associated with increased connectivity between preSMA and rCaudate. In compliance, connection strengths from the rIFG and preSMA into the rCaudate were correlated negatively. If participants failed to stop, the magnitude of experienced interference (Simon effect), but not stopping latency, was predictive for the hyperdirect–indirect model connections. Together, the present results suggest that both the hyperdirect and indirect pathways act together to implement response inhibition, whereas the relationship between performance control and the fronto-basal ganglia connections points toward a top-down mechanism that underlies voluntary action control.

[1]  J. R. Simon Ear preference in a simple reaction-time task. , 1967, Journal of experimental psychology.

[2]  H. Akaike,et al.  Information Theory and an Extension of the Maximum Likelihood Principle , 1973 .

[3]  G. Logan On the ability to inhibit thought and action , 1984 .

[4]  Robert W. Proctor,et al.  Stimulus-Response Compatibility: An Integrated Perspective , 1990 .

[5]  J. Mink THE BASAL GANGLIA: FOCUSED SELECTION AND INHIBITION OF COMPETING MOTOR PROGRAMS , 1996, Progress in Neurobiology.

[6]  Stephen M. Smith,et al.  Temporal Autocorrelation in Univariate Linear Modeling of FMRI Data , 2001, NeuroImage.

[7]  A. Nambu,et al.  Functional significance of the cortico–subthalamo–pallidal ‘hyperdirect’ pathway , 2002, Neuroscience Research.

[8]  Stephen M. Smith,et al.  General multilevel linear modeling for group analysis in FMRI , 2003, NeuroImage.

[9]  T. Robbins,et al.  Inhibitory control in rats performing a stop-signal reaction-time task: effects of lesions of the medial striatum and d-amphetamine. , 2003, Behavioral neuroscience.

[10]  Mark W. Woolrich,et al.  Multilevel linear modelling for FMRI group analysis using Bayesian inference , 2004, NeuroImage.

[11]  David R. Anderson,et al.  Understanding AIC and BIC in Model Selection , 2004 .

[12]  Wei Li,et al.  Larger deficits in brain networks for response inhibition than for visual selective attention in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). , 2005, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[13]  W. Notebaert,et al.  Effects of stimulus-stimulus compatibility and stimulus-response compatibility on response inhibition. , 2005, Acta psychologica.

[14]  Geert J. M. van Boxtel,et al.  Stimulation of the Subthalamic Region Facilitates the Selection and Inhibition of Motor Responses in Parkinson's Disease , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[15]  Laura Busse,et al.  Electrophysiological activity underlying inhibitory control processes in normal adults , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[16]  R. Poldrack,et al.  Cortical and Subcortical Contributions to Stop Signal Response Inhibition: Role of the Subthalamic Nucleus , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[17]  O. Hikosaka,et al.  Switching from automatic to controlled action by monkey medial frontal cortex , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[18]  C. Kennard,et al.  The role of the pre-supplementary motor area in the control of action , 2007, NeuroImage.

[19]  Timothy Edward John Behrens,et al.  Triangulating a Cognitive Control Network Using Diffusion-Weighted Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) and Functional MRI , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[20]  Rajita Sinha,et al.  Subcortical processes of motor response inhibition during a stop signal task , 2008, NeuroImage.

[21]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Striatum and pre-SMA facilitate decision-making under time pressure , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[22]  Birte U. Forstmann,et al.  Neural Mechanisms, Temporal Dynamics, and Individual Differences in Interference Control , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[23]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Function and Structure of the Right Inferior Frontal Cortex Predict Individual Differences in Response Inhibition: A Model-Based Approach , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[24]  Stewart H. Mostofsky,et al.  Response Inhibition and Response Selection: Two Sides of the Same Coin , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[25]  Jiji Zhang,et al.  On the completeness of orientation rules for causal discovery in the presence of latent confounders and selection bias , 2008, Artif. Intell..

[26]  O. Hikosaka,et al.  Role for Subthalamic Nucleus Neurons in Switching from Automatic to Controlled Eye Movement , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  M. Bellgrove,et al.  Insights into the neural basis of response inhibition from cognitive and clinical neuroscience , 2009, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[28]  G. Logan,et al.  Models of response inhibition in the stop-signal and stop-change paradigms , 2009, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[29]  C. Li,et al.  Behavioral/systems/cognitive Functional Connectivity Delineates Distinct Roles of the Inferior Frontal Cortex and Presupplementary Motor Area in Stop Signal Inhibition , 2022 .

[30]  Mark W. Woolrich,et al.  Bayesian analysis of neuroimaging data in FSL , 2009, NeuroImage.

[31]  Michael A. DiSano,et al.  Intracranial EEG Reveals a Time- and Frequency-Specific Role for the Right Inferior Frontal Gyrus and Primary Motor Cortex in Stopping Initiated Responses , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[32]  A. Aron,et al.  Theta burst stimulation dissociates attention and action updating in human inferior frontal cortex , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[33]  Frederick Verbruggen,et al.  Responding with Restraint: What Are the Neurocognitive Mechanisms? , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[34]  Ethan R. Buch,et al.  A Network Centered on Ventral Premotor Cortex Exerts Both Facilitatory and Inhibitory Control over Primary Motor Cortex during Action Reprogramming , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[35]  Maneesh C. Patel,et al.  Distinct frontal systems for response inhibition, attentional capture, and error processing , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[36]  Ethan R. Buch,et al.  Cortical and subcortical interactions during action reprogramming and their related white matter pathways , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[37]  Scott D. Brown,et al.  Cortico-striatal connections predict control over speed and accuracy in perceptual decision making , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[38]  Borís Burle,et al.  Mechanisms and Dynamics of Cortical Motor Inhibition in the Stop-signal Paradigm: A TMS Study , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[39]  B. Forstmann,et al.  Neurocognitive mechanisms of action control: resisting the call of the Sirens. , 2011, Wiley interdisciplinary reviews. Cognitive science.

[40]  Lourens J. Waldorp,et al.  Effective connectivity of fMRI data using ancestral graph theory: Dealing with missing regions , 2011, NeuroImage.

[41]  A. Aron From Reactive to Proactive and Selective Control: Developing a Richer Model for Stopping Inappropriate Responses , 2011, Biological Psychiatry.

[42]  M. Rushworth,et al.  Model-based analyses: Promises, pitfalls, and example applications to the study of cognitive control , 2011, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[43]  B. Hommel,et al.  The Effect of Fmri (noise) on Cognitive Control , 2022 .