A two-platform task reveals a deficit in the ability of rats to return to the start location in the water maze.

The ability of rats to return to the start location was examined with a 4-arm radial water maze. The task required rats to find 2 hidden platforms in sequence. Rats were released from 1 of 3 arms and there was a platform located in the fourth arm. Once a rat found this platform, a 2nd platform was raised in another location, which was either the start location, for 1 group, or another fixed location, for a control group. Across 3 experiments, all rats learned the location of the 1st fixed platform in 80 to 120 trials. However, rats had difficulty finding a 2nd platform if it was at the start location. Control groups revealed that rats could learn 2 platform locations and that the difficulty in learning to return to the start location did not seem to be attributable to its aversive nature. In separate groups, exposure to the start location was increased by starting the rats from an initially stable platform. Rats still did not readily learn to return to the start location. The authors suggest that start location, when varied, cannot readily be used to define the location of a hidden platform.

[1]  SIMON BENHAMOU,et al.  No evidence for cognitive mapping in rats , 1996, Animal Behaviour.

[2]  R. Muller,et al.  Head-direction cells recorded from the postsubiculum in freely moving rats. II. Effects of environmental manipulations , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[3]  Simon Benhamou,et al.  How to find one's way in the labyrinth of path integration models , 1995 .

[4]  SIMON BENHAMOU,et al.  Path integration by swimming rats , 1997, Animal Behaviour.

[5]  B. Poucet Spatial cognitive maps in animals: new hypotheses on their structure and neural mechanisms. , 1993, Psychological review.

[6]  T. Collett,et al.  Local and global vectors in desert ant navigation , 1998, Nature.

[7]  L. Nadel,et al.  The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map , 1978 .

[8]  J. Taube,et al.  Effects of repeated disorientation on the acquisition of spatial tasks in rats: dissociation between the appetitive radial arm maze and aversive water maze. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[9]  K. Jeffery,et al.  Directional control of hippocampal place fields , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[10]  David S. Touretzky,et al.  Neural Representation of Space Using Sinusoidal Arrays , 1993, Neural Computation.

[11]  B. McNaughton,et al.  Hippocampectomized rats are capable of homing by path integration. , 1999, Behavioral neuroscience.

[12]  K. Cheng A purely geometric module in the rat's spatial representation , 1986, Cognition.

[13]  E. Bullmore,et al.  Society for Neuroscience Abstracts , 1997 .

[14]  B. McNaughton,et al.  Place cells, head direction cells, and the learning of landmark stability , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[15]  W E Skaggs,et al.  Deciphering the hippocampal polyglot: the hippocampus as a path integration system. , 1996, The Journal of experimental biology.

[16]  K. Lashley The Mechanism of Vision: III. The Comparative Visual Acuity of Pigmented and Albino Rats , 1930 .

[17]  S. Healy Spatial representation in animals. , 1998 .

[18]  G. M. Martin,et al.  Spatial disorientation blocks reliable goal location on a plus maze but does not prevent goal location in the Morris maze. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[19]  C. Gallistel The organization of learning , 1990 .

[20]  F. Restle Discrimination of cues in mazes: a resolution of the place-vs.-response question. , 1957, Psychological review.

[21]  I Q Whishaw,et al.  Visits to starts, routes, and places by rats (Rattus norvegicus) in swimming pool navigation tasks. , 1986, Journal of comparative psychology.

[22]  A. Etienne,et al.  The effect of a single light cue on homing behaviour of the golden hamster , 1990, Animal Behaviour.

[23]  Simon Benhamou,et al.  A model for place navigation in mammals , 1995 .

[24]  I. Whishaw Place Learning in Hippocampal Rats and the Path Integration Hypothesis , 1998, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[25]  D S Touretzky,et al.  Theory of rodent navigation based on interacting representations of space , 1996, Hippocampus.

[26]  S. Alyan Evidence against instantaneous transfer of spatial knowledge in the house mouse (Mus musculus) , 1994, Psychobiology.

[27]  I. Whishaw,et al.  Piloting and dead reckoning dissociated by fimbria-fornix lesions in a rat food carrying task , 1997, Behavioural Brain Research.

[28]  Ofer Tchernichovski,et al.  Constraints and the Emergence of 'Free' Exploratory Behavior in Rat Ontogeny , 1996 .

[29]  Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al.  A geometric process for spatial reorientation in young children , 1994, Nature.

[30]  S. Glickman,et al.  VISUAL CLIFF BEHAVIOR IN ALBINO AND HOODED RATS. , 1964, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[31]  R. Sutherland,et al.  Some limitations on the use of distal cues in place navigation by rats , 1987, Psychobiology.

[32]  A. Etienne,et al.  Twofold path integration during hoarding in the golden hamster , 1991 .

[33]  Roland Maurer,et al.  A connectionist model of path integration with and without a representation of distance to the starting point , 1998, Psychobiology.

[34]  J. O’Keefe,et al.  Geometric determinants of the place fields of hippocampal neurons , 1996, Nature.

[35]  I. Whishaw Latent learning in a Swimming Pool Place Task by Rats: Evidence for the use of Associative and not Cognitive Mapping Processes , 1991, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.

[36]  A Amsel,et al.  Hippocampal function in the rat: Cognitive mapping or vicarious trial and error? , 1993, Hippocampus.

[37]  A. Etienne,et al.  Role of dead reckoning in navigation , 1998 .

[38]  G. M. Martin,et al.  Multiple points of entry into a circular enclosure prevent place learning despite normal vestibular orientation and cue arrays: evidence for map resetting. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[39]  Rudolf Jander,et al.  Short-range homing in the house mouse, Mus musculus: stages in the learning of directions , 1994, Animal Behaviour.

[40]  L. Abraham,et al.  Vestibular involvement in a passive transport and return task , 1983 .

[41]  I Q Whishaw,et al.  Calibrating space: Exploration is important for allothetic and idiothetic navigation , 1999, Hippocampus.

[42]  H. Blodgett,et al.  Spatial learning in the T-maze; the influence of direction, turn, and food location. , 1949, Journal of experimental psychology.

[43]  R. Sutherland,et al.  Configural association theory: The role of the hippocampal formation in learning, memory, and amnesia , 1989, Psychobiology.

[44]  David Eilam,et al.  Stopping behavior: constraints on exploration in rats (Rattus norvegicus) , 1993, Behavioural Brain Research.

[45]  W. S. Hunter The temporal maze and kinaesthetic sensory processes in the white rat. , 1920 .

[46]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Heading in the rat: Determination by environmental shape , 1988 .

[47]  D. Touretzky,et al.  Cognitive maps beyond the hippocampus , 1997, Hippocampus.

[48]  H. Eichenbaum,et al.  Cues that hippocampal place cells encode: Dynamic and hierarchical representation of local and distal stimuli , 1997, Hippocampus.

[49]  Roland Maurer,et al.  What is modelling for? a critical review of the models of path integration , 1995 .