From Recognizing Known Words to Learning New Ones: Comparing Online Speech Processing in Typically Developing and Late-Talking 2-Year-Olds.

PURPOSE This study examines online speech processing in typically developing and late-talking 2-year-old children, comparing both groups' word recognition, word prediction, and word learning. METHOD English-acquiring U.S. children, from the "When to Worry" study of language and social-emotional development, were identified as typical talkers (n = 67, M age = 27.0 months, SD = 1.4; Study 1) or late talkers (n = 30, M age = 27.0 months, SD = 2.0; Study 2). Children completed an eye-tracking task assessing their ability to recognize both nouns and verbs, to use verbs to predict an upcoming noun's referent, and to use verbs to infer the meaning of novel nouns. RESULTS Both typical and late talkers recognized nouns and verbs and used familiar verbs to predict the referents of upcoming nouns, whether the noun was familiar ("You can eat the apple") or novel ("You can eat the dax"). Late talkers were slower in using familiar nouns to orient to the target and were both slower and less accurate in using familiar verbs to identify the upcoming noun's referent. Notably, however, both groups learned and retained novel word meanings with similar success. CONCLUSIONS Late talkers demonstrated slower lexical processing, especially for verbs. Yet, their success in using familiar verbs to learn novel nouns suggests that, as a group, their slower processing did not impair word learning in this task. This sets the foundation for future work investigating whether these measures predict later language outcomes and can differentiate late talkers with transient delays from those with language disorders.

[1]  L. Wakschlag,et al.  Social EEG: A novel neurodevelopmental approach to studying brain-behavior links and brain-to-brain synchrony during naturalistic toddler-parent interactions. , 2022, Developmental psychobiology.

[2]  J. Saffran,et al.  Predictive language processing in young autistic children , 2022, Autism research : official journal of the International Society for Autism Research.

[3]  K. McGregor,et al.  What Children with Developmental Language Disorder Teach Us About Cross‐Situational Word Learning , 2022, Cogn. Sci..

[4]  S. Arunachalam,et al.  Lexical Processing of Nouns and Verbs at 36 Months of Age Predicts Concurrent and Later Vocabulary and School Readiness , 2020, Journal of cognition and development : official journal of the Cognitive Development Society.

[5]  Viridiana L. Benitez,et al.  The temporal structure of naming events differentially affects children's and adults' cross-situational word learning. , 2020, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[6]  J. Trueswell,et al.  Familiar words can serve as a semantic seed for syntactic bootstrapping. , 2020, Developmental science.

[7]  Cynthia Fisher,et al.  The Developmental Origins of Syntactic Bootstrapping , 2019, Top. Cogn. Sci..

[8]  J. Pine,et al.  Does speed of processing or vocabulary size predict later language growth in toddlers? , 2019, Cognitive Psychology.

[9]  Arielle Borovsky,et al.  Modeling Early Lexico-Semantic Network Development: Perceptual Features Matter Most , 2019, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[10]  Rachel M. Flynn,et al.  Future Directions for Early Childhood Prevention of Mental Disorders: A Road Map to Mental Health, Earlier , 2019, Journal of clinical child and adolescent psychology : the official journal for the Society of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, American Psychological Association, Division 53.

[11]  Michael C. Frank,et al.  childes-db: A flexible and reproducible interface to the child language data exchange system , 2019, Behavior research methods.

[12]  Courtney E. Venker,et al.  Thinking Ahead: Incremental Language Processing is Associated with Receptive Language Abilities in Preschoolers with Autism Spectrum Disorder , 2018, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders.

[13]  Peng Zhou,et al.  Predictive Language Processing in Preschool Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder: An Eye-Tracking Study , 2018, Journal of Psycholinguistic Research.

[14]  Jessica F. Schwab,et al.  Discontinuity of Reference Hinders Children's Learning of New Words. , 2018, Child development.

[15]  Tristan J. Mahr,et al.  Using language input and lexical processing to predict vocabulary size. , 2018, Developmental science.

[16]  Jill Lany Lexical-processing efficiency leverages novel word learning in infants and toddlers. , 2018, Developmental science.

[17]  S. Waxman,et al.  When Veps Cry: Two-Year-Olds Efficiently Learn Novel Words from Linguistic Contexts Alone , 2018, Language learning and development : the official journal of the Society for Language Development.

[18]  Katherine E. Twomey,et al.  Learned Labels Shape Pre‐speech Infants’ Object Representations , 2017, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[19]  Per B. Brockhoff,et al.  lmerTest Package: Tests in Linear Mixed Effects Models , 2017 .

[20]  Steven G. Luke,et al.  Evaluating significance in linear mixed-effects models in R , 2016, Behavior Research Methods.

[21]  R. Aslin,et al.  Semantic Specificity in One-Year-Olds’ Word Comprehension , 2017, Language learning and development : the official journal of the Society for Language Development.

[22]  Haley A. Vlach,et al.  Remember dax? Relations between children's cross-situational word learning, memory, and language abilities. , 2017, Journal of memory and language.

[23]  Eliana Colunga,et al.  Not Only Size Matters: Early-Talker and Late-Talker Vocabularies Support Different Word-Learning Biases in Babies and Networks. , 2017, Cognitive science.

[24]  Julia L. Evans,et al.  Semantic Structure in Vocabulary Knowledge Interacts With Lexical and Sentence Processing in Infancy. , 2016, Child development.

[25]  Arielle Borovsky,et al.  Lexical leverage: category knowledge boosts real-time novel word recognition in 2-year-olds. , 2016, Developmental science.

[26]  Jessica F. Schwab,et al.  Repetition across successive sentences facilitates young children's word learning. , 2016, Developmental psychology.

[27]  V. Shafer,et al.  Vocabulary of Toddlers Who Are Late Talkers , 2016 .

[28]  Arielle Borovsky,et al.  Novel word learning: An eye-tracking study. Are 18-month-old late talkers really different from their typical peers? , 2015, Journal of communication disorders.

[29]  Nikole D. Patson,et al.  Comprehending the impossible: what role do selectional restriction violations play? , 2015, Language, cognition and neuroscience.

[30]  Mary Alt,et al.  Cross-situational statistically based word learning intervention for late-talking toddlers. , 2014, Journal of communication disorders.

[31]  D. Bates,et al.  Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4 , 2014, 1406.5823.

[32]  S. Waxman,et al.  Infants use known verbs to learn novel nouns: Evidence from 15- and 19-month-olds , 2014, Cognition.

[33]  Kim Plunkett,et al.  What’s in a link: Associative and taxonomic priming effects in the infant lexicon , 2013, Cognition.

[34]  Scott P. Johnson,et al.  Memory constraints on infants’ cross-situational statistical learning , 2013, Cognition.

[35]  D. Barr,et al.  Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. , 2013, Journal of memory and language.

[36]  R. Schwartz,et al.  Influence of phonotactic probability/neighbourhood density on lexical learning in late talkers. , 2013, International journal of language & communication disorders.

[37]  Michael C. Frank,et al.  Social and Discourse Contributions to the Determination of Reference in Cross-Situational Word Learning , 2013 .

[38]  B. MacWhinney,et al.  Effect of verb argument structure on picture naming in children with and without specific language impairment (SLI). , 2012, International journal of language & communication disorders.

[39]  J. Elman,et al.  Knowing a lot for one's age: Vocabulary skill and not age is associated with anticipatory incremental sentence interpretation in children and adults. , 2012, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[40]  Falk Huettig,et al.  Prediction during language processing is a piece of cake--but only for skilled producers. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[41]  Joan Guàrdia-Olmos,et al.  Auditory word recognition of nouns and verbs in children with Specific Language Impairment (SLI). , 2012, Journal of communication disorders.

[42]  Anne Fernald,et al.  Individual differences in lexical processing at 18 months predict vocabulary growth in typically developing and late-talking toddlers. , 2012, Child development.

[43]  L. Rescorla Late talkers: do good predictors of outcome exist? , 2011, Developmental disabilities research reviews.

[44]  A. Hofman,et al.  Examining continuity of early expressive vocabulary development: the generation R study. , 2011, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[45]  L. Gleitman,et al.  How words can and cannot be learned by observation , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[46]  Katharine Graf Estes,et al.  Phonotactic constraints on infant word learning. , 2011, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[47]  M. Prior,et al.  Predicting Language Outcomes at 4 Years of Age: Findings From Early Language in Victoria Study , 2010, Pediatrics.

[48]  Sylvia Yuan,et al.  Syntactic bootstrapping. , 2010, Wiley interdisciplinary reviews. Cognitive science.

[49]  Leslie Rescorla,et al.  Age 17 language and reading outcomes in late-talking toddlers: support for a dimensional perspective on language delay. , 2009, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[50]  D. Thal,et al.  Early Language Delay and Risk for Language Impairment , 2008 .

[51]  S. Zubrick,et al.  Language outcomes of 7-year-old children with or without a history of late language emergence at 24 months. , 2008, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[52]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  Infants rapidly learn word-referent mappings via cross-situational statistics , 2008, Cognition.

[53]  M. Prior,et al.  Predicting Language at 2 Years of Age: A Prospective Community Study , 2007, Pediatrics.

[54]  David W. Slegers,et al.  Late language emergence at 24 months: an epidemiological study of prevalence, predictors, and covariates. , 2007, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[55]  Julia L. Evans,et al.  Longitudinal relationships between lexical and grammatical development in typical and late-talking children. , 2007, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[56]  Pamela A Hadley,et al.  Assessing the emergence of grammar in toddlers at risk for specific language impairment. , 2006, Seminars in speech and language.

[57]  M. Eriksson,et al.  Can severely language delayed 3-year-olds be identified at 18 months? Evaluation of a screening version of the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories. , 2006, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[58]  Amy Perfors,et al.  Picking up speed in understanding: Speech processing efficiency and vocabulary growth across the 2nd year. , 2006, Developmental psychology.

[59]  Van Geert Classification of developmental language disorders. , 2004 .

[60]  Susan S. Jones,et al.  Late talkers show no shape bias in a novel name extension task , 2003 .

[61]  Robert Plomin,et al.  Outcomes of early language delay: I. Predicting persistent and transient language difficulties at 3 and 4 years. , 2003, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[62]  Leslie Rescorla,et al.  Language and reading outcomes to age 9 in late-talking toddlers. , 2002, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[63]  B. MacWhinney The Childes Project: Tools for Analyzing Talk, Volume II: the Database , 2000 .

[64]  L. Mcdonough,et al.  The role of semantic context and memory in the acquisition of novel nouns. , 1998, Child development.

[65]  Rhea Paul,et al.  Speech-Language Pathology 10-1997 Reading and Metaphonological Outcomes in Late Talkers , 2018 .

[66]  L. Rescorla,et al.  Late talkers at 2: outcome at age 3. , 1997, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[67]  S. Ellis Weismer,et al.  Lexical learning by children with specific language impairment: effects of linguistic input presented at varying speaking rates. , 1996, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[68]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Variability in early communicative development. , 1994, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[69]  L. Rescorla,et al.  The Language Development Survey: a screening tool for delayed language in toddlers. , 1989, The Journal of speech and hearing disorders.