Post-saccadic location judgments reveal remapping of saccade targets to non-foveal locations.

The present study addresses the question of how objects are localized across saccades. In a task requiring participants to compare the location of a post-saccadic probe with the pre-saccadic target, we investigated the roles of saccade landing site and post-saccadic probe location. Saccade landing sites vary from trial to trial because of oculomotor error but can also be shifted by saccadic adaptation. Visual targets were extinguished during the saccade and reappeared after a short blank to counteract saccadic suppression of displacement. Performance in localizing targets after unadapted saccades was nearly veridical and independent of actual landing site, showing that trial-to-trial oculomotor error did not contribute to post-saccadic localization. This result suggests that much of the oculomotor error of saccades is included in the efference copy vector and this allows the recovery of a remapped target location that is often not foveal, but stable and accurate across trials. Displacement judgments relative to this remapped location will be independent of trial-to-trial variability in landing site. After adapted saccades, post-saccadic localization shifted in the direction opposite to adaptation but again, trial-by-trial landing site variability did not correlate with performance. This result suggests that the efference copy matches the planned upcoming saccade, be it adapted or not.

[1]  Thérèse Collins,et al.  The use of recurrent signals about adaptation for subsequent saccade programming depends on object structure , 2006, Brain Research.

[2]  E Kowler,et al.  Illusory shifts in visual direction accompany adaptation of saccadic eye movements , 1999, Nature.

[3]  R. Wurtz,et al.  What the brain stem tells the frontal cortex. II. Role of the SC-MD-FEF pathway in corollary discharge. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  H. Deubel,et al.  Saccade target selection and object recognition: Evidence for a common attentional mechanism , 1996, Vision Research.

[5]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[6]  Markus Lappe,et al.  Effect of saccadic adaptation on localization of visual targets. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  A. Fuchs,et al.  The characteristics and neuronal substrate of saccadic eye movement plasticity , 2004, Progress in Neurobiology.

[8]  Markus Lappe,et al.  Motor space structures perceptual space: Evidence from human saccadic adaptation , 2007, Brain Research.

[9]  B. Bridgeman,et al.  Immediate post-saccadic information mediates space constancy , 1998, Vision Research.

[10]  B. Bridgeman A review of the role of efference copy in sensory and oculomotor control systems , 1995, Annals of Biomedical Engineering.

[11]  Maria Concetta Morrone,et al.  Influence of saccadic adaptation on spatial localization: comparison of verbal and pointing reports. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[12]  B. Bridgeman,et al.  Postsaccadic target blanking prevents saccadic suppression of image displacement , 1996, Vision Research.

[13]  S. C. Mclaughlin Parametric adjustment in saccadic eye movements , 1967 .

[14]  Bruce Bridgeman,et al.  Failure to detect displacement of the visual world during saccadic eye movements , 1975, Vision Research.

[15]  Ronald A. Rensink,et al.  Change-blindness as a result of ‘mudsplashes’ , 1999, Nature.

[16]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[17]  Ralf Engbert Microsaccades: A microcosm for research on oculomotor control, attention, and visual perception. , 2006, Progress in brain research.

[18]  Frans W Cornelissen,et al.  The Eyelink Toolbox: Eye tracking with MATLAB and the Psychophysics Toolbox , 2002, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[19]  H. Deubel Localization of targets across saccades: Role of landmark objects , 2004 .