The Impact of Depression and Apathy on Sensory Language

The debate that language strongly influences thought is equally met by those who suggest language does not influence thought. While historically, the ability to communicate with words was believed to be intimately tied to an ability to form thoughts, we would argue that thought and language are linked together through our sensory and motor systems and severely impacted by depression and apathy. We test this by conducting parts of speech analysis from the comparative longitudinal studies of two highly creative and prolific writers, where one is diagnosed with Alzheimer’s disease (AD), and the other lives a long and healthy life. We calculate function and contents word ratios, measure lexical repetition, and the use of sensory-based words in textual language to test for depression and apathy in AD which is supported by Mann-Whitney U-Testing and Principal Component Analysis. Our results support the hypothesis that thought and language are impacted by depression and apathy and revealed in a person’s writing style 12 years before a formal diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease presents. We find higher lexical repetition in language 12 years prior to one author being diagnosed with AD while not apparent in the other. We identify low olfactory word use and also find that an increased use of sensory-based adjectives might be a sign of the early onset of Alzheimer’s disease.

[1]  Diane Pecher,et al.  A sharp image or a sharp knife: norms for the modality-exclusivity of 774 concept-property items , 2010, Behavior research methods.

[2]  Patrick Juola,et al.  Cross-Linguistic Stylometric Features : A Preliminary Investigation , 2016 .

[3]  Jeffery L. Ramirez Suicide: Across the Life Span. , 2016, The Nursing clinics of North America.

[4]  M. Mesulam,et al.  Primary progressive aphasia--a language-based dementia. , 2003, The New England journal of medicine.

[5]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  Concept Representation Reflects Multimodal Abstraction: A Framework for Embodied Semantics. , 2016, Cerebral cortex.

[6]  Rom Harré,et al.  The Construction and Deconstruction of Self in Alzheimer's Disease , 1992, Ageing and Society.

[7]  Narun Pornpattananangkul,et al.  Creativity and sensory gating indexed by the P50: Selective versus leaky sensory gating in divergent thinkers and creative achievers , 2015, Neuropsychologia.

[8]  Nancy Johnson,et al.  Preservation of Reasoning in Primary Progressive Aphasia: Further Differentiation from Alzheimer's Disease and the Behavioral Presentation of Frontotemporal Dementia , 2004, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[9]  D. Wesson,et al.  Should olfactory dysfunction be used as a biomarker of Alzheimer’s disease? , 2010, Expert review of neurotherapeutics.

[10]  Miroslav Kubat,et al.  Vocabulary Richness Measure in Genres , 2013, J. Quant. Linguistics.

[11]  Graeme Hirst,et al.  Longitudinal detection of dementia through lexical and syntactic changes in writing: a case study of three British novelists , 2011, Lit. Linguistic Comput..

[12]  Matthew A. Lambon Ralph,et al.  The Rise and Fall of Frequency and Imageability: Noun and Verb Production in Semantic Dementia , 2000, Brain and Language.

[13]  Michael C. Corballis,et al.  The evolution of language: Sharing our mental lives , 2017, Journal of Neurolinguistics.

[14]  Muhammad Imran Khan,et al.  Is the war on terror induced-post traumatic stress disorder; the cause of suicide attack? An approach from psycho-cognitive and neurobiological perspective , 2016 .

[15]  Pablo Moscato,et al.  An Information Theoretic Clustering Approach for Unveiling Authorship Affinities in Shakespearean Era Plays and Poems , 2014, PloS one.

[16]  A. Wilson,et al.  Iris Murdoch As I Knew Her , 2003 .

[17]  Richard Burns,et al.  Business Research Methods and Statistics Using SPSS , 2008 .

[18]  A. Serretti,et al.  Polymorphism of rs3813034 in Serotonin Transporter Gene SLC6A4 Is Associated With the Selective Serotonin and Serotonin-Norepinephrine Reuptake Inhibitor Response in Depressive Disorder: Sequencing Analysis of SLC6A4 , 2016, Journal of clinical psychopharmacology.

[19]  I. Croy,et al.  Olfaction as a marker for depression in humans. , 2014, Journal of affective disorders.

[20]  Michael A Covington,et al.  Automatic measurement of propositional idea density from part-of-speech tagging , 2008, Behavior research methods.

[21]  Alison Ferguson,et al.  Propositional Idea Density in women's written language over the lifespan: Computerized analysis , 2014, Cortex.

[22]  Kumiko Tanaka-Ishii,et al.  Computational Constancy Measures of Texts—Yule’s K and Rényi’s Entropy , 2015, CL.

[23]  Peter Garrard,et al.  Cognitive archaeology: Uses, methods, and results , 2009, Journal of Neurolinguistics.

[24]  J. Bayley Elegy for Iris , 1999 .

[25]  Walter Kintsch,et al.  Reading rate and retention as a function of the number of propositions in the base structure of sentences , 1973 .

[26]  J. Hodges,et al.  The effects of very early Alzheimer's disease on the characteristics of writing by a renowned author. , 2004, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[27]  K. Heilman,et al.  Creative Innovation: Possible Brain Mechanisms , 2003, Neurocase.

[28]  Christopher D. Manning,et al.  Enriching the Knowledge Sources Used in a Maximum Entropy Part-of-Speech Tagger , 2000, EMNLP.

[29]  Graeme Hirst,et al.  Vocabulary Changes in Agatha Christie's Mysteries as an Indication of Dementia: A Case Study , 2009 .

[30]  P. Sah,et al.  Norepinephrine and Corticotropin-Releasing Hormone: Partners in the Neural Circuits that Underpin Stress and Anxiety , 2015, Neuron.

[31]  I. Murdoch Living on Paper: Letters from Iris Murdoch, 1934-1995 , 2015 .

[32]  P. Szot Elevated Cerebrospinal Fluid Norepinephrine in the Elderly can Link Depressionand A Reduced Glymphatic System as Risk Factors for AlzheimerâÂÂs Disease , 2016 .

[33]  P. Lieberman The evolution of language and thought. , 2016, Journal of anthropological sciences = Rivista di antropologia : JASS.

[34]  Evelina Fedorenko,et al.  Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences Language and Thought Are Not the Same Thing: Evidence from Neuroimaging and Neurological Patients , 2022 .

[35]  D R Wekstein,et al.  Linguistic ability in early life and cognitive function and Alzheimer's disease in late life. Findings from the Nun Study. , 1996, JAMA.

[36]  Peter Garrard,et al.  From hindsight to insight – retrospective analysis of language written by a renowned Alzheimer's patient , 2008 .

[37]  H. Meltzer,et al.  Reduced Levels of Norepinephrine Transporters in the Locus Coeruleus in Major Depression , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[38]  Dermot Lynott,et al.  Modality exclusivity norms for 423 object properties , 2009, Behavior research methods.

[39]  Anja Jamrozik,et al.  Metaphor: Bridging embodiment to abstraction , 2016, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[40]  Sofie Van Gijsel,et al.  A variationist, corpus linguistic analysis of lexical richness in Flanders , 2005 .

[41]  Sarah A. Chau,et al.  Apathy and Attentional Biases in Alzheimer's Disease. , 2016, Journal of Alzheimer's disease : JAD.

[42]  Jennifer Weuve,et al.  Cognitive impairment 18 years before clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer disease dementia , 2015, Neurology.

[43]  Kumiko Tanaka-Ishii,et al.  Study on Constants of Natural Language Texts , 2011 .

[44]  A. Vermeer Coming to grips with lexical richness in spontaneous speech data , 2000 .

[45]  John Bayley,et al.  Iris : A Memoir of Iris Murdoch , 2001 .