Mechanisms of Practice-Related Reductions of Dual-Task Interference with Simple Tasks: Data and Theory

In dual-task situations, interference between two simultaneous tasks impairs performance. With practice, however, this impairment can be reduced. To identify mechanisms leading to a practice-related improvement in sensorimotor dual tasks, the present review applied the following general hypothesis: Sources that impair dual-task performance at the beginning of practice are associated with mechanisms for the reduction of dual-task impairment at the end of practice. The following types of processes provide sources for the occurrence of this impairment: (a) capacity-limited processes within the component tasks, such as response-selection or motor response stages, and (b) cognitive control processes independent of these tasks and thus operating outside of component-task performance. Dual-task practice studies show that, under very specific conditions, capacity-limited processes within the component tasks are automatized with practice, reducing the interference between two simultaneous tasks. Further, there is evidence that response-selection stages are shortened with practice. Thus, capacity limitations at these stages are sources for dual-task costs at the beginning of practice and are overcome with practice. However, there is no evidence demonstrating the existence of practice-related mechanisms associated with capacity-limited motor-response stages. Further, during practice, there is an acquisition of executive control skills for an improved allocation of limited attention resources to two tasks as well as some evidence supporting the assumption of improved task coordination. These latter mechanisms are associated with sources of dual-task interference operating outside of component task performance at the beginning of practice and also contribute to the reduction of dual-task interference at its end.

[1]  J. C. Johnston,et al.  Locus of the single-channel bottleneck in dual-task interference , 1992 .

[2]  Torsten Schubert,et al.  Multiple bottlenecks in information processing? An electrophysiological examination , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[3]  R. D. de Jong,et al.  Multiple bottlenecks in overlapping task performance. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[4]  J. E. Korteling Effects of Skill Integration and Perceptual Competition on Age-Related Differences in Dual-Task Performance , 1991, Human factors.

[5]  M. J. Emerson,et al.  The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and Their Contributions to Complex “Frontal Lobe” Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.

[6]  Torsten Schubert,et al.  Processing differences between simple and choice reactions affect bottleneck localization in overlapping tasks , 1999 .

[7]  Pierre Jolicoeur,et al.  Virtually no evidence for virtually perfect time-sharing. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[8]  Eliot Hazeltine,et al.  Simultaneous dual-task performance reveals parallel response selection after practice. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[9]  Rolf Ulrich,et al.  The source of execution-related dual-task interference: motor bottleneck or response monitoring? , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[10]  A. Miyake,et al.  The Nature and Organization of Individual Differences in Executive Functions , 2012, Current directions in psychological science.

[11]  Arthur F. Kramer,et al.  Training for executive control: Task coordination strategies and aging , 1999 .

[12]  Torsten Schubert,et al.  Practice-related optimization and transfer of executive functions: a general review and a specific realization of their mechanisms in dual tasks , 2014, Psychological Research.

[13]  John F. Larish,et al.  Training for attentional control in dual task settings: A comparison of young and old adults , 1995 .

[14]  J. C. Johnston,et al.  Why practice reduces dual-task interference. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[15]  Torsten Schubert,et al.  Testing the Limits of Optimizing Dual-Task Performance in Younger and Older Adults , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[16]  S. Belleville,et al.  Identifying training modalities to improve multitasking in older adults , 2014, AGE.

[17]  E Ruthruff,et al.  Can practice eliminate the psychological refractory period effect? , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[18]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Attention and Effort , 1973 .

[19]  M. Sigman,et al.  Dynamics of the Central Bottleneck: Dual-Task and Task Uncertainty , 2006, PLoS biology.

[20]  Torsten Schubert,et al.  Neural mechanisms of concurrent stimulus processing in dual tasks , 2009, NeuroImage.

[21]  Scott Watter,et al.  PRP training shows Task1 response selection is the locus of the backward response compatibility effect , 2015, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[22]  E. Spelke,et al.  Dividing Attention Without Alternation or Automaticity , 1980 .

[23]  J. C. Johnston,et al.  Chronometric Evidence for Central Postponement in Temporally Overlapping Tasks , 2003 .

[24]  S. Hochstein,et al.  Attentional Demands Following Perceptual Skill Training , 2001, Psychological science.

[25]  E. Ruthruff,et al.  Learning to bypass the central bottleneck: declining automaticity with advancing age. , 2010, Psychology and aging.

[26]  Torsten Schubert,et al.  Response activation in overlapping tasks and the response-selection bottleneck. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[27]  D Gopher,et al.  Different difficulty manipulations interact differently with task emphasis: evidence for multiple resources. , 1982, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[28]  Klaus Oberauer,et al.  Simultaneous cognitive operations in working memory after dual-task practice. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[29]  J. C. Johnston,et al.  Vanishing dual-task interference after practice: has the bottleneck been eliminated or is it merely latent? , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[30]  A. Kramer,et al.  Transfer Effects in Task-Set Cost and Dual-Task Cost After Dual-Task Training in Older and Younger Adults: Further Evidence for Cognitive Plasticity in Attentional Control in Late Adulthood , 2008, Experimental aging research.

[31]  H. Zimmer,et al.  Cognitive Science 2007 : Proceedings of the 8th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society of Germany , 2008 .

[32]  Torsten Schubert,et al.  Effects of extensive dual-task practice on processing stages in simultaneous choice tasks , 2013, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[33]  H. Müller,et al.  Age- and practice-related influences on dual-task costs and compensation mechanisms under optimal conditions of dual-task performance , 2012, Neuropsychology, development, and cognition. Section B, Aging, neuropsychology and cognition.

[34]  Torsten Schubert,et al.  Improved Intertask Coordination after Extensive Dual-Task Practice , 2011, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[35]  David E. Kieras,et al.  A computational theory of executive cognitive processes and multiple-task performance: Part 2. Accounts of psychological refractory-period phenomena. , 1997 .

[36]  Arthur F Kramer,et al.  Training effects on dual-task performance: are there age-related differences in plasticity of attentional control? , 2005, Psychology and aging.

[37]  R. Schmidt,et al.  New Conceptualizations of Practice: Common Principles in Three Paradigms Suggest New Concepts for Training , 1992 .

[38]  Bernhard Hommel,et al.  Control of stimulus-response translation in dual-task performance , 2002, Psychological research.

[39]  Camarin E. Rolle,et al.  Video game training enhances cognitive control in older adults , 2013, Nature.

[40]  P. Jolicoeur,et al.  A central capacity sharing model of dual-task performance. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[41]  Walter R. Boot,et al.  Effects of training strategies implemented in a complex videogame on functional connectivity of attentional networks , 2012, NeuroImage.

[42]  Torsten Schubert,et al.  Video game practice optimizes executive control skills in dual-task and task switching situations. , 2012, Acta psychologica.

[43]  C. W. Telford The refractory phase of voluntary and associative responses , 1931 .

[44]  Ritske de Jong,et al.  THE ROLE OF PREPARATION IN OVERLAPPING-TASK PERFORMANCE , 1995 .

[45]  A. Kramer,et al.  Videogame training strategy-induced change in brain function during a complex visuomotor task , 2012, Behavioural Brain Research.

[46]  T. Schubert,et al.  Does Initial Performance Variability Predict Dual-Task Optimization with Practice in Younger and Older Adults? , 2015, Experimental aging research.

[47]  S. Belleville,et al.  Training of attentional control in mild cognitive impairment with executive deficits: Results from a double-blind randomised controlled study , 2012, Neuropsychological rehabilitation.

[48]  R. Proctor,et al.  Task switching and response correspondence in the psychological refractory period paradigm. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[49]  H. Pashler,et al.  The specificity of learned parallelism in dual-memory retrieval , 2014, Memory & cognition.

[50]  Torsten Schubert,et al.  Better dual-task processing in simultaneous interpreters , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[51]  Ritske de Jong,et al.  Multiple bottlenecks in overlapping task performance , 1993 .

[52]  A. Hartley,et al.  A demonstration of dual-task performance without interference in some older adults. , 2011, Psychology and aging.

[53]  Torsten Schubert,et al.  On the importance of Task 1 and error performance measures in PRP dual-task studies , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[54]  G. Logan Toward an instance theory of automatization. , 1988 .

[55]  Roy Luria,et al.  Online order control in the psychological refractory period paradigm. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[56]  Walter Schneider,et al.  Controlled and Automatic Human Information Processing: 1. Detection, Search, and Attention. , 1977 .

[57]  H. Pashler,et al.  Procedural learning. I, Locus of practice effects in speeded choice tasks , 1991 .

[58]  F. Tong,et al.  Training Improves Multitasking Performance by Increasing the Speed of Information Processing in Human Prefrontal Cortex , 2009, Neuron.

[59]  R. D. Gordon,et al.  Executive control of visual attention in dual-task situations. , 2001, Psychological review.

[60]  A. Welford THE ‘PSYCHOLOGICAL REFRACTORY PERIOD’ AND THE TIMING OF HIGH‐SPEED PERFORMANCE—A REVIEW AND A THEORY , 1952 .

[61]  Maxi Becker,et al.  Simultaneous interpreters vs. professional multilingual controls: Group differences in cognitive control as well as brain structure and function , 2016, NeuroImage.

[62]  Christopher D. Wickens,et al.  The identification and transfer of timesharing skills , 1980 .

[63]  D E Kieras,et al.  A computational theory of executive cognitive processes and multiple-task performance: Part 1. Basic mechanisms. , 1997, Psychological review.

[64]  H Pashler,et al.  Does perceptual analysis continue during selection and production of a speeded response? , 1995, Acta psychologica.

[65]  G. Band,et al.  Reconfiguration and the bottleneck: Does task switching affect the refractory period effect? , 2006 .

[66]  B. Hommel Automatic stimulus-response translation in dual-task performance. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[67]  H. Masaki,et al.  An electrophysiological study of the locus of the interference effect in a stimulus-response compatibility paradigm. , 2000, Psychophysiology.

[68]  Arthur F Kramer,et al.  Testing the limits of cognitive plasticity in older adults: application to attentional control. , 2006, Acta psychologica.

[69]  J. Requin Attention and Performance VII , 1980 .

[70]  D. Navon,et al.  Queuing or Sharing? A Critical Evaluation of the Single-Bottleneck Notion , 2002, Cognitive Psychology.

[71]  J. C. Johnston,et al.  Can practice eliminate the psychological refractory period effect , 1999 .

[72]  François Maquestiaux,et al.  Can practice overcome age-related differences in the psychological refractory period effect? , 2004, Psychology and aging.

[73]  Christopher D. Wickens,et al.  Multiple Resources and Mental Workload , 2008, Hum. Factors.

[74]  Torsten Schubert,et al.  Investigation on the improvement and transfer of dual-task coordination skills , 2012, Psychological research.

[75]  Torsten Schubert,et al.  Practice-related reduction of dual-task costs under conditions of a manual-pedal response combination , 2011 .

[76]  Nachshon Meiran,et al.  Working memory involvement in dual-task performance: Evidence from the backward compatibility effect , 2008, Memory & cognition.

[77]  Torsten Schubert,et al.  The central attentional limitation and executive control. , 2008, Frontiers in bioscience : a journal and virtual library.

[78]  P. Hampson Aspects of Attention and Cognitive Science , 1989 .

[79]  Daniel Gopher,et al.  On the Economy of the Human Processing System: A Model of Multiple Capacity. , 1977 .

[80]  Torsten Schubert,et al.  Task-order coordination in dual-task performance and the lateral prefrontal cortex: an event-related fMRI study , 2006, Psychological research.

[81]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Provides New Constraints on Theories of the Psychological Refractory Period , 2004, Psychological science.

[82]  H. Müller,et al.  Evidence for the acquisition of dual-task coordination skills in older adults. , 2015, Acta psychologica.

[83]  Klaus Oberauer,et al.  Eliminating dual-task costs by minimizing crosstalk between tasks: The role of modality and feature pairings , 2016, Cognition.

[84]  J. C. Johnston,et al.  How does practice reduce dual-task interference: Integration, automatization, or just stage-shortening? , 2006, Psychological research.

[85]  Torsten Schubert,et al.  Functional neuroanatomy of interference in overlapping dual tasks: an fMRI study. , 2003, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[86]  P. Dux,et al.  Transfer of training benefits requires rules we cannot see (or hear). , 2016, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[87]  Wilfried Kunde,et al.  Who is talking in backward crosstalk? Disentangling response- from goal-conflict in dual-task performance , 2014, Cognition.

[88]  Jennifer M. Glass,et al.  Virtually Perfect Time Sharing in Dual-Task Performance: Uncorking the Central Cognitive Bottleneck , 2001, Psychological science.

[89]  H. Pashler Dual-task interference in simple tasks: data and theory. , 1994, Psychological bulletin.

[90]  Lost ability to automatize task performance in old age , 2013, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[91]  Rolf Ulrich,et al.  On the optimality of serial and parallel processing in the psychological refractory period paradigm: Effects of the distribution of stimulus onset asynchronies , 2009, Cognitive Psychology.

[92]  John R. Anderson,et al.  Learning to achieve perfect timesharing: architectural implications of Hazeltine, Teague, and Ivry (2002). , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[93]  J. C. Johnston,et al.  Attentional limitations in dual-task performance. , 1998 .

[94]  L. Bherer,et al.  An Investigation of Response and Stimulus Modality Transfer Effects after Dual-Task Training in Younger and Older , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[95]  Daniel G Bobrow,et al.  On data-limited and resource-limited processes , 1975, Cognitive Psychology.

[96]  Harold Pashler,et al.  Effects of practice on task architecture: Combined evidence from interference experiments and random-walk models of decision making , 2011, Cognition.

[97]  H Pashler,et al.  Processing stages in overlapping tasks: evidence for a central bottleneck. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[98]  Eric Ruthruff,et al.  Bypassing the central bottleneck after single-task practice in the psychological refractory period paradigm: Evidence for task automatization and greedy resource recruitment , 2008, Memory & cognition.

[99]  W. Sommer,et al.  Localizing practice effects in dual-task performance , 2007, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[100]  P. Dux,et al.  The influence of training on the attentional blink and psychological refractory period , 2014, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.

[101]  D. Chiappe,et al.  Improving multi-tasking ability through action videogames. , 2013, Applied ergonomics.

[102]  G. Logan,et al.  Parallel response selection in dual-task situations , 2006, Perception & psychophysics.

[103]  P. Dux,et al.  Training conquers multitasking costs by dividing task representations in the frontoparietal-subcortical system , 2015, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[104]  Ritske De Jong,et al.  The Role of Preparation in Overlapping-task Performance , 1995, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[105]  H. Pashler,et al.  Graded capacity-sharing in dual-task interference? , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[106]  Rico Fischer,et al.  Efficient multitasking: parallel versus serial processing of multiple tasks , 2015, Front. Psychol..