Mechanisms of Practice-Related Reductions of Dual-Task Interference with Simple Tasks: Data and Theory
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] J. C. Johnston,et al. Locus of the single-channel bottleneck in dual-task interference , 1992 .
[2] Torsten Schubert,et al. Multiple bottlenecks in information processing? An electrophysiological examination , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[3] R. D. de Jong,et al. Multiple bottlenecks in overlapping task performance. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[4] J. E. Korteling. Effects of Skill Integration and Perceptual Competition on Age-Related Differences in Dual-Task Performance , 1991, Human factors.
[5] M. J. Emerson,et al. The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and Their Contributions to Complex “Frontal Lobe” Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.
[6] Torsten Schubert,et al. Processing differences between simple and choice reactions affect bottleneck localization in overlapping tasks , 1999 .
[7] Pierre Jolicoeur,et al. Virtually no evidence for virtually perfect time-sharing. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[8] Eliot Hazeltine,et al. Simultaneous dual-task performance reveals parallel response selection after practice. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[9] Rolf Ulrich,et al. The source of execution-related dual-task interference: motor bottleneck or response monitoring? , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[10] A. Miyake,et al. The Nature and Organization of Individual Differences in Executive Functions , 2012, Current directions in psychological science.
[11] Arthur F. Kramer,et al. Training for executive control: Task coordination strategies and aging , 1999 .
[12] Torsten Schubert,et al. Practice-related optimization and transfer of executive functions: a general review and a specific realization of their mechanisms in dual tasks , 2014, Psychological Research.
[13] John F. Larish,et al. Training for attentional control in dual task settings: A comparison of young and old adults , 1995 .
[14] J. C. Johnston,et al. Why practice reduces dual-task interference. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[15] Torsten Schubert,et al. Testing the Limits of Optimizing Dual-Task Performance in Younger and Older Adults , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[16] S. Belleville,et al. Identifying training modalities to improve multitasking in older adults , 2014, AGE.
[17] E Ruthruff,et al. Can practice eliminate the psychological refractory period effect? , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[18] D. Kahneman,et al. Attention and Effort , 1973 .
[19] M. Sigman,et al. Dynamics of the Central Bottleneck: Dual-Task and Task Uncertainty , 2006, PLoS biology.
[20] Torsten Schubert,et al. Neural mechanisms of concurrent stimulus processing in dual tasks , 2009, NeuroImage.
[21] Scott Watter,et al. PRP training shows Task1 response selection is the locus of the backward response compatibility effect , 2015, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[22] E. Spelke,et al. Dividing Attention Without Alternation or Automaticity , 1980 .
[23] J. C. Johnston,et al. Chronometric Evidence for Central Postponement in Temporally Overlapping Tasks , 2003 .
[24] S. Hochstein,et al. Attentional Demands Following Perceptual Skill Training , 2001, Psychological science.
[25] E. Ruthruff,et al. Learning to bypass the central bottleneck: declining automaticity with advancing age. , 2010, Psychology and aging.
[26] Torsten Schubert,et al. Response activation in overlapping tasks and the response-selection bottleneck. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[27] D Gopher,et al. Different difficulty manipulations interact differently with task emphasis: evidence for multiple resources. , 1982, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[28] Klaus Oberauer,et al. Simultaneous cognitive operations in working memory after dual-task practice. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[29] J. C. Johnston,et al. Vanishing dual-task interference after practice: has the bottleneck been eliminated or is it merely latent? , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[30] A. Kramer,et al. Transfer Effects in Task-Set Cost and Dual-Task Cost After Dual-Task Training in Older and Younger Adults: Further Evidence for Cognitive Plasticity in Attentional Control in Late Adulthood , 2008, Experimental aging research.
[31] H. Zimmer,et al. Cognitive Science 2007 : Proceedings of the 8th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society of Germany , 2008 .
[32] Torsten Schubert,et al. Effects of extensive dual-task practice on processing stages in simultaneous choice tasks , 2013, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[33] H. Müller,et al. Age- and practice-related influences on dual-task costs and compensation mechanisms under optimal conditions of dual-task performance , 2012, Neuropsychology, development, and cognition. Section B, Aging, neuropsychology and cognition.
[34] Torsten Schubert,et al. Improved Intertask Coordination after Extensive Dual-Task Practice , 2011, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[35] David E. Kieras,et al. A computational theory of executive cognitive processes and multiple-task performance: Part 2. Accounts of psychological refractory-period phenomena. , 1997 .
[36] Arthur F Kramer,et al. Training effects on dual-task performance: are there age-related differences in plasticity of attentional control? , 2005, Psychology and aging.
[37] R. Schmidt,et al. New Conceptualizations of Practice: Common Principles in Three Paradigms Suggest New Concepts for Training , 1992 .
[38] Bernhard Hommel,et al. Control of stimulus-response translation in dual-task performance , 2002, Psychological research.
[39] Camarin E. Rolle,et al. Video game training enhances cognitive control in older adults , 2013, Nature.
[40] P. Jolicoeur,et al. A central capacity sharing model of dual-task performance. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[41] Walter R. Boot,et al. Effects of training strategies implemented in a complex videogame on functional connectivity of attentional networks , 2012, NeuroImage.
[42] Torsten Schubert,et al. Video game practice optimizes executive control skills in dual-task and task switching situations. , 2012, Acta psychologica.
[43] C. W. Telford. The refractory phase of voluntary and associative responses , 1931 .
[44] Ritske de Jong,et al. THE ROLE OF PREPARATION IN OVERLAPPING-TASK PERFORMANCE , 1995 .
[45] A. Kramer,et al. Videogame training strategy-induced change in brain function during a complex visuomotor task , 2012, Behavioural Brain Research.
[46] T. Schubert,et al. Does Initial Performance Variability Predict Dual-Task Optimization with Practice in Younger and Older Adults? , 2015, Experimental aging research.
[47] S. Belleville,et al. Training of attentional control in mild cognitive impairment with executive deficits: Results from a double-blind randomised controlled study , 2012, Neuropsychological rehabilitation.
[48] R. Proctor,et al. Task switching and response correspondence in the psychological refractory period paradigm. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[49] H. Pashler,et al. The specificity of learned parallelism in dual-memory retrieval , 2014, Memory & cognition.
[50] Torsten Schubert,et al. Better dual-task processing in simultaneous interpreters , 2015, Front. Psychol..
[51] Ritske de Jong,et al. Multiple bottlenecks in overlapping task performance , 1993 .
[52] A. Hartley,et al. A demonstration of dual-task performance without interference in some older adults. , 2011, Psychology and aging.
[53] Torsten Schubert,et al. On the importance of Task 1 and error performance measures in PRP dual-task studies , 2015, Front. Psychol..
[54] G. Logan. Toward an instance theory of automatization. , 1988 .
[55] Roy Luria,et al. Online order control in the psychological refractory period paradigm. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[56] Walter Schneider,et al. Controlled and Automatic Human Information Processing: 1. Detection, Search, and Attention. , 1977 .
[57] H. Pashler,et al. Procedural learning. I, Locus of practice effects in speeded choice tasks , 1991 .
[58] F. Tong,et al. Training Improves Multitasking Performance by Increasing the Speed of Information Processing in Human Prefrontal Cortex , 2009, Neuron.
[59] R. D. Gordon,et al. Executive control of visual attention in dual-task situations. , 2001, Psychological review.
[60] A. Welford. THE ‘PSYCHOLOGICAL REFRACTORY PERIOD’ AND THE TIMING OF HIGH‐SPEED PERFORMANCE—A REVIEW AND A THEORY , 1952 .
[61] Maxi Becker,et al. Simultaneous interpreters vs. professional multilingual controls: Group differences in cognitive control as well as brain structure and function , 2016, NeuroImage.
[62] Christopher D. Wickens,et al. The identification and transfer of timesharing skills , 1980 .
[63] D E Kieras,et al. A computational theory of executive cognitive processes and multiple-task performance: Part 1. Basic mechanisms. , 1997, Psychological review.
[64] H Pashler,et al. Does perceptual analysis continue during selection and production of a speeded response? , 1995, Acta psychologica.
[65] G. Band,et al. Reconfiguration and the bottleneck: Does task switching affect the refractory period effect? , 2006 .
[66] B. Hommel. Automatic stimulus-response translation in dual-task performance. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[67] H. Masaki,et al. An electrophysiological study of the locus of the interference effect in a stimulus-response compatibility paradigm. , 2000, Psychophysiology.
[68] Arthur F Kramer,et al. Testing the limits of cognitive plasticity in older adults: application to attentional control. , 2006, Acta psychologica.
[69] J. Requin. Attention and Performance VII , 1980 .
[70] D. Navon,et al. Queuing or Sharing? A Critical Evaluation of the Single-Bottleneck Notion , 2002, Cognitive Psychology.
[71] J. C. Johnston,et al. Can practice eliminate the psychological refractory period effect , 1999 .
[72] François Maquestiaux,et al. Can practice overcome age-related differences in the psychological refractory period effect? , 2004, Psychology and aging.
[73] Christopher D. Wickens,et al. Multiple Resources and Mental Workload , 2008, Hum. Factors.
[74] Torsten Schubert,et al. Investigation on the improvement and transfer of dual-task coordination skills , 2012, Psychological research.
[75] Torsten Schubert,et al. Practice-related reduction of dual-task costs under conditions of a manual-pedal response combination , 2011 .
[76] Nachshon Meiran,et al. Working memory involvement in dual-task performance: Evidence from the backward compatibility effect , 2008, Memory & cognition.
[77] Torsten Schubert,et al. The central attentional limitation and executive control. , 2008, Frontiers in bioscience : a journal and virtual library.
[78] P. Hampson. Aspects of Attention and Cognitive Science , 1989 .
[79] Daniel Gopher,et al. On the Economy of the Human Processing System: A Model of Multiple Capacity. , 1977 .
[80] Torsten Schubert,et al. Task-order coordination in dual-task performance and the lateral prefrontal cortex: an event-related fMRI study , 2006, Psychological research.
[81] N. Kanwisher,et al. Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Provides New Constraints on Theories of the Psychological Refractory Period , 2004, Psychological science.
[82] H. Müller,et al. Evidence for the acquisition of dual-task coordination skills in older adults. , 2015, Acta psychologica.
[83] Klaus Oberauer,et al. Eliminating dual-task costs by minimizing crosstalk between tasks: The role of modality and feature pairings , 2016, Cognition.
[84] J. C. Johnston,et al. How does practice reduce dual-task interference: Integration, automatization, or just stage-shortening? , 2006, Psychological research.
[85] Torsten Schubert,et al. Functional neuroanatomy of interference in overlapping dual tasks: an fMRI study. , 2003, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[86] P. Dux,et al. Transfer of training benefits requires rules we cannot see (or hear). , 2016, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[87] Wilfried Kunde,et al. Who is talking in backward crosstalk? Disentangling response- from goal-conflict in dual-task performance , 2014, Cognition.
[88] Jennifer M. Glass,et al. Virtually Perfect Time Sharing in Dual-Task Performance: Uncorking the Central Cognitive Bottleneck , 2001, Psychological science.
[89] H. Pashler. Dual-task interference in simple tasks: data and theory. , 1994, Psychological bulletin.
[90] Lost ability to automatize task performance in old age , 2013, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[91] Rolf Ulrich,et al. On the optimality of serial and parallel processing in the psychological refractory period paradigm: Effects of the distribution of stimulus onset asynchronies , 2009, Cognitive Psychology.
[92] John R. Anderson,et al. Learning to achieve perfect timesharing: architectural implications of Hazeltine, Teague, and Ivry (2002). , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[93] J. C. Johnston,et al. Attentional limitations in dual-task performance. , 1998 .
[94] L. Bherer,et al. An Investigation of Response and Stimulus Modality Transfer Effects after Dual-Task Training in Younger and Older , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[95] Daniel G Bobrow,et al. On data-limited and resource-limited processes , 1975, Cognitive Psychology.
[96] Harold Pashler,et al. Effects of practice on task architecture: Combined evidence from interference experiments and random-walk models of decision making , 2011, Cognition.
[97] H Pashler,et al. Processing stages in overlapping tasks: evidence for a central bottleneck. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[98] Eric Ruthruff,et al. Bypassing the central bottleneck after single-task practice in the psychological refractory period paradigm: Evidence for task automatization and greedy resource recruitment , 2008, Memory & cognition.
[99] W. Sommer,et al. Localizing practice effects in dual-task performance , 2007, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[100] P. Dux,et al. The influence of training on the attentional blink and psychological refractory period , 2014, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.
[101] D. Chiappe,et al. Improving multi-tasking ability through action videogames. , 2013, Applied ergonomics.
[102] G. Logan,et al. Parallel response selection in dual-task situations , 2006, Perception & psychophysics.
[103] P. Dux,et al. Training conquers multitasking costs by dividing task representations in the frontoparietal-subcortical system , 2015, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[104] Ritske De Jong,et al. The Role of Preparation in Overlapping-task Performance , 1995, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[105] H. Pashler,et al. Graded capacity-sharing in dual-task interference? , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[106] Rico Fischer,et al. Efficient multitasking: parallel versus serial processing of multiple tasks , 2015, Front. Psychol..