Silex: A database for silent-letter endings in French words

Silent-letter endings are often claimed to be a major source of inconsistency in the French orthography. In this report, we introduce Silex, a database designed to facilitate the study of spelling performance in general, and silent-letter endings in particular. It was derived from two large and recent corpora based on child- and adult-targeted material. Silex consists of three kinds of Excel workbooks: a set of Stimuli Selector workbooks that allow researchers to select words based on a variety of statistics and word characteristics; a Table Generator workbook that allows researchers to build consistency distribution tables by selecting specific phonological or orthographic units; and a Master File workbook, from which all statistics were derived, and that allows researchers to compute other statistics. Silex is different from existing databases in the manner that silent-letter endings were coded and how consistency indices were computed. Importantly, Silex provides unconditional- and conditional-consistency indices for silent-letter endings. To demonstrate the utility of Silex, we first described the silent-letter phenomenon in French. We found that, at minimum, 28 % of French words end with a silent letter. Moreover, silent-letter endings are usually t, e, s, x, or d, and the occurrence of these letters is conditioned by the phonological ending of words. Second, we showed how Silex could prove useful for the development of theoretical models and for empirical studies. The novel information provided in Silex as well as the flexibility of this database should enable researchers to advance our understanding of developing and skilled spelling performance.

[1]  Bernard Lété,et al.  MANULEX: A grade-level lexical database from French elementary school readers , 2004, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[2]  Bernard Lété,et al.  Manulex-infra: Distributional characteristics of grapheme—phoneme mappings, and infralexical and lexical units in child-directed written material , 2007, Behavior research methods.

[3]  R. Peereman,et al.  Consistency and word-frequency effects on spelling among first-to fifth-grade French children : A regression-based study , 2008 .

[4]  M. Sénéchal Morphological effects in children's spelling of French words. , 2000, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[5]  P. Mousty,et al.  Brulex: une base de donne 'es lexicales informatise 'e pour le franc?ais e 'crit et parle , 1990 .

[6]  Michel Hupet,et al.  The acquisition of subject-verb agreement in written French: From novices to experts' errors , 1999 .

[7]  Sébastien Pacton,et al.  The timing and mechanisms of children's use of morphological information in spelling: A review of evidence from English and French , 2008 .

[8]  P. Barrouillet,et al.  Disentangling the impact of semantic and formal factors in the acquisition of number inflections: Noun, adjective and verb agreement in written French , 2006 .

[9]  Arthur M. Jacobs,et al.  Statistical analysis of the bidirectional inconsistency of spelling and sound in French , 1996 .

[10]  Rebecca Treiman,et al.  Learning to Spell from Reading: General Knowledge about Spelling Patterns Influences Memory for Specific Words , 2014, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[11]  Marco Zorzi,et al.  When silent letters say more than a thousand words: An implementation and evaluation of CDP++ in French , 2014 .

[12]  Marc Brysbaert,et al.  Comparing Word Processing Times in Naming, Lexical Decision, and Progressive Demasking: Evidence from Chronolex , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[13]  R. Peereman,et al.  LEXOP: A lexical database providing orthography-phonology statistics for French monosyllabic words , 1999, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[14]  J. Jaffré Introduction: The Orthography of French , 2005 .

[15]  R. Treiman,et al.  Context sensitivity in the spelling of English vowels , 2002 .

[16]  M. Sénéchal,et al.  Morphological knowledge as revealed in children's spelling accuracy and reports of spelling strategies. , 2006, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[17]  P. Perruchet,et al.  Children's implicit learning of graphotactic and morphological regularities. , 2005, Child development.

[18]  Isabelle Bonnotte,et al.  Statistical learning of past participle inflections in French , 2013, Reading and Writing.

[19]  Rebecca Treiman,et al.  How Children Learn to Write Words , 2014 .

[20]  Marc Brysbaert,et al.  Lexique 2 : A new French lexical database , 2004, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[21]  R. Peereman,et al.  The contribution of morphology to the consistency of spelling-to-sound relations: A quantitative analysis based on French elementary school readers , 2013, L’Année psychologique.

[22]  André Porquet,et al.  L'orthographe française , 1966 .

[23]  J. Ziegler,et al.  Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: a psycholinguistic grain size theory. , 2005, Psychological bulletin.

[24]  Maxime Gingras,et al.  Modeling Spelling Acquisition: The Effect of Orthographic Regularities on Silent-Letter Representations , 2016 .

[25]  The moderating effect of orthographic consistency on oral vocabulary learning in monolingual and bilingual children. , 2014, Journal of experimental child psychology.