Dissociation between unconscious motor response facilitation and conflict in medial frontal areas

Masked prime tasks have shown that sensory information that has not been consciously perceived can nevertheless modulate behavior. The neuronal correlates of behavioral manifestations of visuomotor priming remain debated, particularly with respect to the distribution and direction (i.e. increase or decrease) of activity changes in medial frontal areas. Here, we predicted that these discrepant results could be accounted for by two automatic and unconscious processes embedded in this task: response conflict and facilitation. We used event‐related functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), as 24 healthy participants had to respond, as fast as possible, to a target arrow presented immediately after a subliminal masked prime arrow. There were three experimental conditions defined by the prime–target relationship: compatible, incompatible, and neutral. The classical visuomotor priming effect was reproduced, with relatively longer reaction times (RTs) in incompatible trials. Longer RTs in incompatible than in neutral trials were specifically associated with stronger blood oxygen level‐dependent (BOLD) activity in a conflict‐related network comprising the anterior cingulate cortex and right frontal associative areas. Motor response facilitation as shown by shorter RTs in compatible than in neutral trials was associated with reduced activation in a motor preparation network including the medial and lateral premotor cortices, as a result of the repetition suppression of the fMRI BOLD signal. The present results provide new insights into automatic and unconscious visuomotor priming processes, suggesting an involvement of either a cognitive or motor network, depending on the prime–target relationship.

[1]  K. D'ostilio,et al.  Automatic Stimulus-Induced Medial Premotor Cortex Activation without Perception or Action , 2011, PloS one.

[2]  Catie Chang,et al.  Neural Basis of Repetition Priming during Mathematical Cognition: Repetition Suppression or Repetition Enhancement? , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[3]  P. Haggard,et al.  Subliminal priming of actions influences sense of control over effects of action , 2010, Cognition.

[4]  Simon van Gaal,et al.  Unconscious Activation of the Prefrontal No-Go Network , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[5]  Emmanuel Dupoux,et al.  Cerebral bases of subliminal speech priming , 2010, NeuroImage.

[6]  J. D. de Fockert,et al.  Unconscious priming of a no-go response. , 2009, Psychophysiology.

[7]  M. Girelli,et al.  Does subliminal visual perception have an error‐monitoring system? , 2009, The European journal of neuroscience.

[8]  Scott T Grafton,et al.  Repetition suppression for performed hand gestures revealed by fMRI , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[9]  Robert L. West,et al.  Positive and negative congruency effects in masked priming: A neuro-computational model based on representation, attention, and conflict , 2009, Brain Research.

[10]  Cameron S. Carter,et al.  Conflict-related activity in the caudal anterior cingulate cortex in the absence of awareness , 2009, Biological Psychology.

[11]  M Husain,et al.  Motor neglect associated with loss of action inhibition , 2008, Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry.

[12]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Frontal Cortex Mediates Unconsciously Triggered Inhibitory Control , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[13]  R. Henson,et al.  Priming, response learning and repetition suppression , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[14]  M. Husain,et al.  Control over Conflict during Movement Preparation: Role of Posterior Parietal Cortex , 2008, Neuron.

[15]  Michael Falkenstein,et al.  ERP—Correlates of response selection in a response conflict paradigm , 2008, Brain Research.

[16]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Cerebral bases of subliminal and supraliminal priming during reading. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[17]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Evidence for a distributed hierarchy of action representation in the brain. , 2007, Human movement science.

[18]  C. Kennard,et al.  Human Medial Frontal Cortex Mediates Unconscious Inhibition of Voluntary Action , 2007, Neuron.

[19]  R. Passingham,et al.  Unconscious Activation of the Cognitive Control System in the Human Prefrontal Cortex , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[20]  T Shallice,et al.  Regional frontal injuries cause distinct impairments in cognitive control , 2007, Neurology.

[21]  J. Jonides,et al.  Interference resolution: Insights from a meta-analysis of neuroimaging tasks , 2007, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[22]  P. Praamstra,et al.  Time-course of masked response priming and inhibition in Parkinson's disease , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[23]  M. Hogge,et al.  Exploration of the neural substrates of executive functioning by functional neuroimaging , 2006, Neuroscience.

[24]  F. Schlaghecken,et al.  Low-level motor inhibition in children : evidence from the negative compatibility effect , 2006 .

[25]  Hakwan C. Lau,et al.  Dissociating response selection and conflict in the medial frontal surface , 2006, NeuroImage.

[26]  J. Stout,et al.  Activation of conflicting responses in Parkinson's disease: evidence for degrading and facilitating effects on response time , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[27]  Tor D. Wager,et al.  Common and unique components of response inhibition revealed by fMRI , 2005, NeuroImage.

[28]  E. Maylor,et al.  Motor control in old age: evidence of impaired low-level inhibition. , 2005, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[29]  Peter Praamstra,et al.  The Neurophysiology of Response Competition: Motor Cortex Activation and Inhibition following Subliminal Response Priming , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[30]  Peter Praamstra,et al.  The basal ganglia and inhibitory mechanisms in response selection: evidence from subliminal priming of motor responses in Parkinson's disease. , 2004, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[31]  R. Henson,et al.  Neural response suppression, haemodynamic repetition effects, and behavioural priming , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[32]  M. Leboyer,et al.  Conscious and subliminal conflicts in normal subjects and patients with schizophrenia: The role of the anterior cingulate , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[33]  T. Robbins,et al.  Inhibition of subliminally primed responses is mediated by the caudate and thalamus: evidence from functional MRI and Huntington's disease. , 2003, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[34]  C. Carter,et al.  The Timing of Action-Monitoring Processes in the Anterior Cingulate Cortex , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[35]  J B Poline,et al.  Cerebral mechanisms of word masking and unconscious repetition priming , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[36]  R. Poldrack,et al.  Neural Activation During Response Competition , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[37]  J. Cohen,et al.  Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. , 2000, Science.

[38]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Conflict monitoring versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulate cortex , 1999, Nature.

[39]  Peter Praamstra,et al.  Motor cortex activation in Parkinson's disease: Dissociation of electrocortical and peripheral measures of response generation , 1999, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[40]  M. Eimer,et al.  Effects of masked stimuli on motor activation: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[41]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Imaging unconscious semantic priming , 1998, Nature.

[42]  B. Kopp,et al.  N2, P3 and the lateralized readiness potential in a nogo task involving selective response priming. , 1996, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[43]  D. Meyer,et al.  A Neural System for Error Detection and Compensation , 1993 .

[44]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Impaired mesial frontal and putamen activation in Parkinson's disease: A positron emission tomography study , 1992, Annals of neurology.

[45]  D. Norman,et al.  Attention to Action: Willed and Automatic Control of Behavior Technical Report No. 8006. , 1980 .

[46]  Russell A Poldrack,et al.  Cognition: Basal Ganglia Role , 2009 .

[47]  W. D. Penny,et al.  Random-Effects Analysis , 2002 .

[48]  Martin Eimer,et al.  Motor activation with and without inhibition: Evidence for a threshold mechanism in motor control , 2002, Perception & psychophysics.

[49]  W. Penny,et al.  Random-Effects Analysis , 2002 .

[50]  O. Neumann Direct parameter specification and the concept of perception , 1990, Psychological research.

[51]  L Weiskrantz,et al.  Visual capacity in the hemianopic field following a restricted occipital ablation. , 1974, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[52]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .