Effects of multitasking‐training on gray matter structure and resting state neural mechanisms

Multitasking (MT) constitutes engaging in two or more cognitive activities at the same time. MT‐training improves performance on untrained MT tasks and alters the functional activity of the brain during MT. However, the effects of MT‐training on neural mechanisms beyond MT‐related functions are not known. We investigated the effects of 4 weeks of MT‐training on regional gray matter volume (rGMV) and functional connectivity during rest (resting‐FC) in young human adults. MT‐training was associated with increased rGMV in three prefrontal cortical regions (left lateral rostral prefrontal cortex (PFC), dorsolateral PFC (DLPFC), and left inferior frontal junction), the left posterior parietal cortex, and the left temporal and lateral occipital areas as well as decreased resting‐FC between the right DLPFC and an anatomical cluster around the ventral anterior cingulate cortex (ACC). Our findings suggest that participation in MT‐training is as a whole associated with task‐irrelevant plasticity (i.e., neural changes are not limited to certain specific task conditions) in regions and the network that are assumed to play roles in MT as well as diverse higher‐order cognitive functions. We could not dissociate the effects of each task component and the diverse cognitive processes involved in MT because of the nature of the study, and these remain to be investigated. Hum Brain Mapp 35:3646–3660, 2014. © 2013 The Authors. Human Brain Mapping Published by Wiley Periodicals, Inc.

[1]  箱田 裕司,et al.  集団用ストループ・逆ストループテスト 反応様式, 順序, 練習の効果 , 1990 .

[2]  Torsten Schubert,et al.  Dissociable Neural Effects of Task Order Control and Task Set Maintenance during Dual-task Processing , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[3]  K. Amunts,et al.  Neural correlates of dual task interference can be dissociated from those of divided attention: an fMRI study. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[4]  Yasuyuki Taki,et al.  Working Memory Training Using Mental Calculation Impacts Regional Gray Matter of the Frontal and Parietal Regions , 2011, PloS one.

[5]  Paige E. Scalf,et al.  Training-induced functional activation changes in dual-task processing: an FMRI study. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[6]  J. Raven,et al.  Manual for Raven's progressive matrices and vocabulary scales , 1962 .

[7]  Yasuyuki Taki,et al.  A voxel-based morphometry study of gray and white matter correlates of a need for uniqueness , 2012, NeuroImage.

[8]  F. Tong,et al.  Training Improves Multitasking Performance by Increasing the Speed of Information Processing in Human Prefrontal Cortex , 2009, Neuron.

[9]  Yasuyuki Taki,et al.  Effects of Working Memory Training on Cognitive Functions and Neural Systems , 2010, Reviews in the neurosciences.

[10]  Maurizio Corbetta,et al.  The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[11]  D. Scott Perceptual learning. , 1974, Queen's nursing journal.

[12]  S. Yantis,et al.  Transient neural activity in human parietal cortex during spatial attention shifts , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[13]  H. Forssberg,et al.  Training of Working Memory in Children With ADHD , 2002 .

[14]  Christa Neuper,et al.  Individual differences in mathematical competence predict parietal brain activation during mental calculation , 2007, NeuroImage.

[15]  Thomas E. Nichols,et al.  Nonstationary cluster-size inference with random field and permutation methods , 2004, NeuroImage.

[16]  Ramon Casanova,et al.  Biological parametric mapping: A statistical toolbox for multimodality brain image analysis , 2007, NeuroImage.

[17]  M. Lorr,et al.  Profile of mood states , 1971 .

[18]  Randall W Engle,et al.  Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: a latent-variable approach. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[19]  J. Binder,et al.  A Parametric Manipulation of Factors Affecting Task-induced Deactivation in Functional Neuroimaging , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[20]  Yasuyuki Taki,et al.  Effects of working memory training on functional connectivity and cerebral blood flow during rest , 2013, Cortex.

[21]  Monica Melby-Lervåg,et al.  Is working memory training effective? A meta-analytic review. , 2013, Developmental psychology.

[22]  Yasuyuki Taki,et al.  Neural Correlates of the Difference between Working Memory Speed and Simple Sensorimotor Speed: An fMRI Study , 2012, PloS one.

[23]  R. Haier,et al.  The Parieto-Frontal Integration Theory (P-FIT) of intelligence: Converging neuroimaging evidence , 2007, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[24]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Detecting Activations in PET and fMRI: Levels of Inference and Power , 1996, NeuroImage.

[25]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  The neural basis of the central executive system of working memory , 1995, Nature.

[26]  Susan Cooley,et al.  Cognitive enhancement therapy for schizophrenia: effects of a 2-year randomized trial on cognition and behavior. , 2004, Archives of general psychiatry.

[27]  P. Fransson Spontaneous low‐frequency BOLD signal fluctuations: An fMRI investigation of the resting‐state default mode of brain function hypothesis , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[28]  D. Dimitrov,et al.  Pretest-posttest designs and measurement of change. , 2003, Work.

[29]  Jan Derrfuss,et al.  Cognitive control in the posterior frontolateral cortex: evidence from common activations in task coordination, interference control, and working memory , 2004, NeuroImage.

[30]  Jun Li,et al.  Brain spontaneous functional connectivity and intelligence , 2008, NeuroImage.

[31]  Alexandra B. Morrison,et al.  Expanding the mind’s workspace: Training and transfer effects with a complex working memory span task , 2010, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[32]  A D Baddeley,et al.  The decline of working memory in Alzheimer's disease. A longitudinal study. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[33]  Ryuta Kawashima,et al.  Effects of processing speed training on cognitive functions and neural systems , 2012, Reviews in the neurosciences.

[34]  M. Brass,et al.  The role of the inferior frontal junction area in cognitive control , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[35]  J. Gabrieli,et al.  Hyperactivity and hyperconnectivity of the default network in schizophrenia and in first-degree relatives of persons with schizophrenia , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[36]  J. Jonides,et al.  Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes. , 1999, Science.

[37]  H. Spinnler The prefrontal cortex, Anatomy, physiology, and neuropsychology of the frontal lobe, J.M. Fuster. Raven Press, New York (1980), IX-222 pages , 1981 .

[38]  Theodore P. Zanto,et al.  Characterizing the Involvement of Rostrolateral Prefrontal Cortex in Prospective Memory , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[39]  B. Dritschel,et al.  Gender differences in the dual-task effects on autobiographical memory retrieval during social problem solving. , 1998, British journal of psychology.

[40]  A. Tachibana,et al.  Activation of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in a dual neuropsychological screening test: An fMRI approach , 2012, Behavioral and Brain Functions.

[41]  Vinod Menon,et al.  Functional connectivity in the resting brain: A network analysis of the default mode hypothesis , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[42]  Mariko Osaka,et al.  Cooperation of the anterior cingulate cortex and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex for attention shifting , 2004, NeuroImage.

[43]  Mark D'Esposito,et al.  Working memory impairments in traumatic brain injury: evidence from a dual-task paradigm , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[44]  Y. Benjamini,et al.  Adaptive linear step-up procedures that control the false discovery rate , 2006 .

[45]  Simon Baron-Cohen,et al.  The Essential Difference: The Truth About the Male and Female Brain , 2003 .

[46]  Christian Büchel,et al.  Changes in Gray Matter Induced by Learning—Revisited , 2008, PloS one.

[47]  F. Paas,et al.  Cognitive Architecture and Instructional Design , 1998 .

[48]  Yasuyuki Taki,et al.  Effects of Training of Processing Speed on Neural Systems , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[49]  Thomas S. Redick,et al.  No evidence of intelligence improvement after working memory training: a randomized, placebo-controlled study. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[50]  Susanne M. Jaeggi,et al.  Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory: a meta-analysis , 2008, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[51]  Yasuyuki Taki,et al.  Cerebral Blood Flow during Rest Associates with General Intelligence and Creativity , 2011, PloS one.

[52]  Yasuyuki Taki,et al.  Regional gray matter volume of dopaminergic system associate with creativity: Evidence from voxel-based morphometry , 2010, NeuroImage.

[53]  R. Engle,et al.  Individual differences in working memory capacity and what they tell us about controlled attention, general fluid intelligence, and functions of the prefrontal cortex. , 1999 .

[54]  Iroise Dumontheil,et al.  The gateway hypothesis of rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10) function , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[55]  H. Forssberg,et al.  Computerized training of working memory in children with ADHD--a randomized, controlled trial. , 2005, Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry.

[56]  M. Posner,et al.  Short-term meditation training improves attention and self-regulation , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[57]  A. Kramer,et al.  Transfer Effects in Task-Set Cost and Dual-Task Cost After Dual-Task Training in Older and Younger Adults: Further Evidence for Cognitive Plasticity in Attentional Control in Late Adulthood , 2008, Experimental aging research.

[58]  C. Wendelken,et al.  Rostrolateral prefrontal cortex: Domain‐general or domain‐sensitive? , 2012, Human brain mapping.

[59]  A. Green,et al.  Methodological issues in the use of the concurrent activities paradigm , 1986, Brain and Cognition.

[60]  Sharona M. Atkins,et al.  Memory enhancement in healthy older adults using a brain plasticity-based training program: A randomized, controlled study , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[61]  Yasuyuki Taki,et al.  Failing to deactivate: The association between brain activity during a working memory task and creativity , 2011, NeuroImage.

[62]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[63]  S. Rombouts,et al.  Consistent resting-state networks across healthy subjects , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[64]  J. Cerella,et al.  Aging and dual-task performance: a meta-analysis. , 2003, Psychology and aging.

[65]  Christian Gaser,et al.  Gray Matter Increase Induced by Practice Correlates with Task-Specific Activation: A Combined Functional and Morphometric Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[66]  C. S. Green,et al.  Action video game modifies visual selective attention , 2003, Nature.

[67]  M. Mirmehdi,et al.  16th Annual Meeting of the Organization for Human Brain Mapping , 2010 .

[68]  R. Haier,et al.  Neuroanatomy of creativity , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[69]  Yasuyuki Taki,et al.  Regional gray and white matter volume associated with Stroop interference: Evidence from voxel-based morphometry , 2012, NeuroImage.

[70]  G. A. Mendelsohn,et al.  Associative and attentional processes in creative performance1 , 1976 .

[71]  Mikkel Wallentin,et al.  Putative sex differences in verbal abilities and language cortex: A critical review , 2009, Brain and Language.