Placing a tool in the spotlight: spatial attention modulates visuomotor responses in cortex
暂无分享,去创建一个
David J. Turk | Michael S. Gazzaniga | Jana Schaich Borg | Scott T. Grafton | Todd C. Handy | Christine M. Tipper | M. Gazzaniga | T. Handy | C. Tipper | D. Turk
[1] A. Treisman,et al. Attention, Space, and Action: Studies in Cognitive Neuroscience , 2001 .
[2] E. Vogel,et al. Sensory gain control (amplification) as a mechanism of selective attention: electrophysiological and neuroimaging evidence. , 1998, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.
[3] D. Boles,et al. An Experimental Comparison of Stimulus Type, Display Type, and Input Variable Contributions to Visual Field Asymmetry , 1994, Brain and Cognition.
[4] R. Ellis,et al. Action priming by briefly presented objects. , 2004, Acta psychologica.
[5] G R Mangun,et al. Spatial distribution of visual attention: Perceptual sensitivity and response latency , 1996, Perception & psychophysics.
[6] G. Mangun. Neural mechanisms of visual selective attention. , 1995, Psychophysiology.
[7] M. Goodale,et al. Visual control of action but not perception requires analytical processing of object shape , 2003, Nature.
[8] Steven A. Hillyard,et al. Mechanisms and models of se-lective attention , 1995 .
[9] J. G. Snodgrass,et al. A standardized set of 260 pictures: norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.
[10] K. D. Singh,et al. Negative BOLD in the visual cortex: Evidence against blood stealing , 2004, Human brain mapping.
[11] G. Humphreys,et al. Detection by action: neuropsychological evidence for action-defined templates in search , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.
[12] M. Corbetta,et al. Common Blood Flow Changes across Visual Tasks: II. Decreases in Cerebral Cortex , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[13] S. Tipper,et al. Selective reaching: evidence for action-centered attention. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[14] N. Kanwisher,et al. Covert visual attention modulates face-specific activity in the human fusiform gyrus: fMRI study. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.
[15] R. Passingham,et al. The left parietal cortex and motor attention , 1997, Neuropsychologia.
[16] P. Strick,et al. Imaging the premotor areas , 2001, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[17] Karl J. Friston,et al. Attention to Action: Specific Modulation of Corticocortical Interactions in Humans , 2001, NeuroImage.
[18] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Action for perception: a motor-visual attentional effect. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[19] Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al. Neural correlates of category-specific knowledge , 1996, Nature.
[20] M. Rushworth,et al. The left parietal and premotor cortices: motor attention and selection , 2003, NeuroImage.
[21] M. Gazzaniga,et al. Combined spatial and temporal imaging of brain activity during visual selective attention in humans , 1994, Nature.
[22] P. Fox,et al. Retinotopic organization of early visual spatial attention effects as revealed by PET and ERPs , 1997, Human brain mapping.
[23] J. Bullier,et al. Anatomical segregation of two cortical visual pathways in the macaque monkey , 1990, Visual Neuroscience.
[24] B Giesbrecht,et al. Neural mechanisms of top-down control during spatial and feature attention , 2003, NeuroImage.
[25] M. Jeannerod. Neural Simulation of Action: A Unifying Mechanism for Motor Cognition , 2001, NeuroImage.
[26] Richard T. Dyde,et al. Two illusions of perceived orientation: one fools all of the people some of the time; the other fools all of the people all of the time , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.
[27] Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al. Object vision and spatial vision: two cortical pathways , 1983, Trends in Neurosciences.
[28] Karl J. Friston,et al. Spatial registration and normalization of images , 1995 .
[29] J B Poline,et al. Partially overlapping neural networks for real and imagined hand movements. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.
[30] Karl J. Friston,et al. Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: A general linear approach , 1994 .
[31] G. Luppino,et al. Parietofrontal Circuits for Action and Space Perception in the Macaque Monkey , 2001, NeuroImage.
[32] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Space and selective attention , 1994 .
[33] R. Passingham,et al. The Attentional Role of the Left Parietal Cortex: The Distinct Lateralization and Localization of Motor Attention in the Human Brain , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[34] John L Bradshaw,et al. Asymmetries in preparation for action , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[35] Scott T. Grafton,et al. Premotor Cortex Activation during Observation and Naming of Familiar Tools , 1997, NeuroImage.
[36] R. Passingham,et al. Objects automatically potentiate action: an fMRI study of implicit processing , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.
[37] J R Hodges,et al. "What" and "how": evidence for the dissociation of object knowledge and mechanical problem-solving skills in the human brain. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[38] L. Buxbaum,et al. Action matters: The role of action plans and object affordances in selection for action , 2002 .
[39] Alex Martin,et al. Representation of Manipulable Man-Made Objects in the Dorsal Stream , 2000, NeuroImage.
[40] E. Bizzi,et al. The Cognitive Neurosciences , 1996 .
[41] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Reorienting attention across the horizontal and vertical meridians: Evidence in favor of a premotor theory of attention , 1987, Neuropsychologia.
[42] R. Ellis,et al. On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[43] A. B. Mayer,et al. Visuomotor transformations: early cortical mechanisms of reaching , 1998, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[44] Scott T. Grafton,et al. Graspable objects grab attention when the potential for action is recognized , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.
[45] Patrick Haggard,et al. Supplementary motor area provides an efferent signal for sensory suppression. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[46] M. Rugg,et al. Electrophysiology of Mind: Event-Related Brain Potentials and Cognition , 1995 .
[47] M. Gazzaniga,et al. The new cognitive neurosciences , 2000 .
[48] R. Klein,et al. Is Posner's "beam" the same as Treisman's "glue"?: On the relation between visual orienting and feature integration theory. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[49] David B. Boles,et al. Hemispheric Interaction in Visual Field Asymmetry , 1983, Cortex.
[50] G. Humphreys,et al. Seeing the action: neuropsychological evidence for action-based effects on object selection , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.
[51] R. Chua,et al. Influence of terminal action requirements on action-centered distractor effects , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.
[52] M. Goodale,et al. The visual brain in action , 1995 .
[53] M. Corbetta,et al. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[55] C. J. Downing. Expectancy and visual-spatial attention: effects on perceptual quality. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[56] G. R. Mangun,et al. Monitoring the Visual World: Hemispheric Asymmetries and Subcortical Processes in Attention , 1994, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[57] R. E Passingham,et al. Cerebral dominance for action in the human brain: the selection of actions , 2001, Neuropsychologia.
[58] K. Kiehl,et al. Removal of Confounding Effects of Global Signal in Functional MRI Analyses , 2001, NeuroImage.
[59] G R Mangun,et al. Attention and spatial selection: Electrophysiological evidence for modulation by perceptual load , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.
[60] M. Raichle,et al. The neural correlates of consciousness: an analysis of cognitive skill learning. , 1998, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.
[61] E. Marg. A VISION OF THE BRAIN , 1994 .
[62] G. Mangun,et al. The neural mechanisms of top-down attentional control , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.
[63] S. Zeki. A vision of the brain , 1993 .
[64] Howard S. Bashinski,et al. Enhancement of perceptual sensitivity as the result of selectively attending to spatial locations , 1980, Perception & psychophysics.
[65] M. D’Esposito,et al. The Inferential Impact of Global Signal Covariates in Functional Neuroimaging Analyses , 1998, NeuroImage.
[66] Richard Dyde,et al. Why do some perceptual illusions affect visually guided action, when others don't? , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[67] A. T. Smith,et al. Attentional suppression of activity in the human visual cortex , 2000, Neuroreport.
[68] Chris Rorden,et al. Action and perception: Evidence against converging selection processes , 2002 .
[69] Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al. Organization of visual inputs to the inferior temporal and posterior parietal cortex in macaques , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[70] E. DeYoe,et al. Concurrent processing in the primate visual cortex. , 1995 .
[71] H. Deubel,et al. Selective Dorsal and Ventral Processing: Evidence for a Common Attentional Mechanism in Reaching and Perception , 1998 .
[72] A. Shmuel,et al. Sustained Negative BOLD, Blood Flow and Oxygen Consumption Response and Its Coupling to the Positive Response in the Human Brain , 2002, Neuron.
[73] P. Halligan,et al. The relevance of behavioural measures for functional-imaging studies of cognition , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[74] A. Dale,et al. The Retinotopy of Visual Spatial Attention , 1998, Neuron.
[75] F. Lacquaniti,et al. Eye-hand coordination during reaching. I. Anatomical relationships between parietal and frontal cortex. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.
[76] P. Haggard,et al. Localising awareness of action with transcranial magnetic stimulation , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.
[77] Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al. Discrete Cortical Regions Associated with Knowledge of Color and Knowledge of Action , 1995, Science.
[78] Francesco Lacquaniti,et al. Multiple levels of representation of reaching in the parieto-frontal network. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.
[79] M. Moscovitch,et al. Hemispheric control of spatial attention , 1990, Brain and Cognition.
[80] S. Petersen,et al. Characterizing the Hemodynamic Response: Effects of Presentation Rate, Sampling Procedure, and the Possibility of Ordering Brain Activity Based on Relative Timing , 2000, NeuroImage.
[81] Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al. Mechanisms of directed attention in the human extrastriate cortex as revealed by functional MRI. , 1998, Science.
[82] D. Harrington,et al. Hemispheric asymmetry of movement , 1996, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[83] Karl J. Friston,et al. Movement‐Related effects in fMRI time‐series , 1996, Magnetic resonance in medicine.
[84] F. Lacquaniti,et al. Parieto-frontal coding of reaching: an integrated framework , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.
[85] J. Decety,et al. Does visual perception of object afford action? Evidence from a neuroimaging study , 2002, Neuropsychologia.
[86] Jason B. Mattingley,et al. Motor role of human inferior parietal lobe revealed in unilateral neglect patients , 1998, Nature.