Toddlers' Learning From Socially Meaningful Video Characters

Toddlers' performance on a seriation sequencing task was measured after exposure to a video as a function of the social meaningfulness of the character. Forty eight 21-month-old toddlers were randomly assigned to a socially meaningful character video demonstration, a less socially meaningful character video demonstration, or a no exposure control group. Results indicated that toddlers learned the seriation sequencing task better from a video when a socially meaningful character, rather than a less socially meaningful character, demonstrated the task. Our findings demonstrate that toddlers under age two can learn cognitive, logical reasoning skills from a video presentation when the onscreen character is socially meaningful to them.

[1]  Ronda M. Scantlin,et al.  The relations of early television viewing to school readiness and vocabulary of children from low-income families: the early window project. , 2001, Child development.

[2]  Daniel R. Anderson,et al.  Television and Reality: Toddlers' Use of Visual Information from Video to Guide Behavior , 2002 .

[3]  Special issue on the content and context of early media exposure , 2010 .

[4]  Shalom M. Fisch,et al.  A Capacity Model of Children's Comprehension of Educational Content on Television , 2000 .

[5]  D. Fragaszy,et al.  The sources of skill in seriating cups in children, monkeys and apes , 2002 .

[6]  Gabrielle A. Strouse,et al.  "Don't try this at home": toddlers' imitation of new skills from people on video. , 2008, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[7]  Rebekah A. Richert,et al.  Babies, television and videos: How did we get here? , 2010 .

[8]  Rachel Barr,et al.  The effect of repetition on imitation from television during infancy. , 2007, Developmental psychobiology.

[9]  Marina Krcmar,et al.  Can Social Meaningfulness and Repeat Exposure Help Infants and Toddlers Overcome the Video Deficit? , 2010 .

[10]  Jennings Bryant,et al.  Researching Blue's Clues: Viewing Behavior and Impact , 2000 .

[11]  Michael C. Frank,et al.  Development of infants’ attention to faces during the first year , 2009, Cognition.

[12]  J. Steven Reznick,et al.  Short-form versions of the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories , 2000, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[13]  E. E. Conger,et al.  Interaction and Participation for Young Hispanic and Caucasian Girls' and Boys' Learning of Media Content , 2007 .

[14]  K. McGraw,et al.  Forming inferences about some intraclass correlation coefficients. , 1996 .

[15]  V. Rideout,et al.  Media Family: Electronic Media in the Lives of Infants, Toddlers, Preschoolers, and Their Parents, The , 2006 .

[16]  A. Silverstein The Developmental Psychology of Jean Piaget , 1965 .

[17]  Daniel R. Anderson,et al.  Effects of repeated exposures to a single episode of the television program Blue's Clues on the viewing behaviors and comprehension of preschool children. , 1999 .

[18]  Georgene L. Troseth,et al.  Getting a clear picture: young children's understanding of a televised image , 2003 .

[19]  Linda S. Siegel,et al.  Development of the Concept of Seriation. , 1972 .

[20]  Sandra L. Calvert,et al.  Pace and Continuity of Television Programs: Effects on Children's Attention and Comprehension , 2004 .

[21]  H. Hayne,et al.  Developmental changes in imitation from television during infancy. , 1999, Child development.

[22]  Daniel R. Anderson,et al.  Early childhood television viewing and adolescent behavior: the recontact study. , 2001, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[23]  J. Deloache,et al.  The development of error correction strategies in young children's manipulative play. , 1985, Child development.

[24]  J. Kotler,et al.  The Influence of Media Characters on Children's Food Choices , 2012, Journal of health communication.

[25]  Cynthia A. Hoffner Parasocial and Online Social Relationships , 2009 .

[26]  E. Vandewater,et al.  A US Study of Transfer of Learning from Video to Books in Toddlers , 2010 .

[27]  L. S. Vygotksy Mind in society: the development of higher psychological processes , 1978 .

[28]  Richard E. Redding,et al.  Mastery motivation in infants and toddlers: Is it greatest when tasks are moderately challenging? , 1988 .

[29]  D. Horton,et al.  Mass communication and para-social interaction; observations on intimacy at a distance. , 1956, Psychiatry.

[30]  J. Deloache,et al.  The medium can obscure the message: young children's understanding of video. , 1998, Child development.

[31]  B. Zimmerman,et al.  Training Seriation Responses in Young Children through Televised Modeling of Hierarchically Sequenced Rule Components1 , 1975 .

[32]  Georgene L. Troseth,et al.  Is it life or is it Memorex? Video as a representation of reality , 2010 .

[33]  Rachel Barr,et al.  Transfer of learning between 2D and 3D sources during infancy: Informing theory and practice. , 2010, Developmental review : DR.

[34]  Daniel R. Anderson,et al.  Television and Very Young Children , 2005 .

[35]  J. Piaget The construction of reality in the child , 1954 .

[36]  P. Kuhl,et al.  Foreign-language experience in infancy: Effects of short-term exposure and social interaction on phonetic learning , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[37]  Emily Atkinson,et al.  Music interferes with learning from television during infancy , 2010 .

[38]  C. Hoffner Children's wishful identification and parasocial interaction with favorite television characters , 1996 .

[39]  Sandra L. Calvert,et al.  The handbook of children, media, and development , 2008 .

[40]  E. Saltzman,et al.  The development of rulebound strategies for manipulating seriated cups: A parallel between action and grammar , 1972 .

[41]  Georgene L. Troseth,et al.  Young children's use of video as a source of socially relevant information. , 2006, Child development.