Non-spatial neglect for the mental number line

Several psychophysical investigations, expanding the classical introspective observations by Galton, have suggested that the mental representation of numbers takes the form of a number line along which magnitude is positioned in ascending order according to reading habits, i.e. from left to right in Western cultures. In keeping with the evidence, pathological rightward deviations in the bisection of number intervals due to right brain damage are generally interpreted as originating from a purely spatial-attentional deficit in the processing of the left side of number intervals. However, consistent double dissociations between defective processing of the left side of physical and mental number space have called into question the universality of this interpretation. Recent evidence suggests a link between rightward deviations in number space and defective memory for both spatial and non-spatial sequences of items. Here we describe the case of a left brain-damaged patient exhibiting right-sided neglect for extrapersonal and representational space, and left-sided neglect on the mental number line. Accurate neuropsychological examination revealed that the apparent left-sided neglect in the bisection of number intervals had a purely non-spatial origin and was based on mnemonic difficulties for the initial items of verbal sequences presented visually at an identical spatial position. These findings show that effective position-based verbal working memory might be crucial for numerical tasks that are usually considered to involve purely spatial representation of numerical magnitudes.

[1]  K. Priftis,et al.  Brain damage: Neglect disrupts the mental number line , 2002, Nature.

[2]  M. Albert A simple test of visual neglect , 1973, Neurology.

[3]  A. Bartolo,et al.  Left Ipsilesional Neglect for Visual Imagery: A Mental Image Generation Impairment? , 2006, Neurocase.

[4]  M. Brysbaert Arabic number reading: On the nature of the numerical scale and the origin of phonological recoding. , 1995 .

[5]  T. Loetscher,et al.  Lucky numbers: Spatial neglect affects physical, but not representational, choices in a Lotto task , 2010, Cortex.

[6]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[7]  C. Guariglia,et al.  Pure representational neglect and navigational deficits in a case with preserved visuo-spatial working memory , 2008, Neurocase.

[8]  A. Benton,et al.  Visuospatial judgment. A clinical test. , 1978, Archives of neurology.

[9]  HERMAN BUSCHKE,et al.  Evaluating storage, retention, and retrieval in disordered memory and learning , 1974, Neurology.

[10]  Yanghua Tian,et al.  Dissociation between visual line bisection and mental number line bisection in schizophrenia , 2011, Neuroscience Letters.

[11]  Sergio Della Sala,et al.  Perceiving Left and Imagining Right: Dissociation in Neglect , 2000, Cortex.

[12]  Patrik Vuilleumier,et al.  The Number Space and Neglect , 2004, Cortex.

[13]  Jean-Philippe van Dijck,et al.  Numbers are associated with different types of spatial information depending on the task , 2009, Cognition.

[14]  Marco Zorzi,et al.  The spatial representation of numerical and non-numerical sequences: Evidence from neglect , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[15]  S. Dehaene,et al.  THREE PARIETAL CIRCUITS FOR NUMBER PROCESSING , 2003, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[16]  R. Reitan,et al.  The Halstead-Reitan neuropsychological test battery: Theory and clinical interpretation , 1993 .

[17]  S. Dehaene,et al.  The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. , 1993 .

[18]  W. Fias The Importance of Magnitude Information in Numerical Processing: Evidence from the SNARC Effect , 1996 .

[19]  J. Caviness,et al.  Myoclonus of peripheral origin , 1999, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[20]  A. Baddeley,et al.  Pattern span: a tool for unwelding visuo–spatial memory , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[21]  Paul H Garthwaite,et al.  Testing for suspected impairments and dissociations in single-case studies in neuropsychology: evaluation of alternatives using monte carlo simulations and revised tests for dissociations. , 2005, Neuropsychology.

[22]  Paola Guariglia,et al.  Dissociation between physical and mental number line bisection in right hemisphere brain damage , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[23]  W. Sturm,et al.  Neuropsychological assessment , 2007, Journal of Neurology.

[24]  P. Pantano,et al.  Unilateral neglect restricted to visual imagery , 1993, Nature.

[25]  L. Pelosi,et al.  On the different roles of the cerebral hemispheres in mental imagery: The “o'Clock Test” in two clinical cases , 1989, Brain and Cognition.

[26]  R. Logie,et al.  The bailiwick of visuo-spatial working memory: evidence from unilateral spatial neglect. , 1996, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[27]  Paola Guariglia,et al.  Spatial Orienting Biases in the Decimal Numeral System , 2009, Current Biology.

[28]  Margarete Delazer,et al.  The Mental Representation of Ordered Sequences in Visual Neglect , 2007, Cortex.

[29]  Robert H. Logie,et al.  Visual perception and verbal descriptions as sources for generating mental representations: Evidence from representational neglect , 2002, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[30]  L. Gauthier,et al.  The Bells Test: A quantitative and qualitative test for visual neglect. , 1989 .

[31]  M. McCourt,et al.  Pseudoneglect: a review and meta-analysis of performance factors in line bisection tasks , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[32]  Marinella Cappelletti,et al.  The middle house or the middle floor: Bisecting horizontal and vertical mental number lines in neglect , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[33]  B. Wilson,et al.  Development of a behavioral test of visuospatial neglect. , 1987, Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation.

[34]  E. Bisiach,et al.  Unilateral Neglect of Representational Space , 1978, Cortex.

[35]  E. Kaplan,et al.  The Boston naming test , 2001 .

[36]  Marinella Cappelletti,et al.  Dissociations and interactions between time, numerosity and space processing , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[37]  J. Ridley Studies of Interference in Serial Verbal Reactions , 2001 .

[38]  S. Bloch Visuospatial judgment. , 1979, Archives of neurology.

[39]  M. Perenin,et al.  Geographic Information has to be Spatialised to be Neglected: A Representational Neglect Case , 2004, Cortex.

[40]  L. Rapport,et al.  Validation of the Warrington theory of visual processing and the Visual Object and Space Perception Battery. , 1998, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[41]  강연욱,et al.  Symbol Digit Modalities Test(SDMT)의 유용성 , 2007 .

[42]  R. Logie,et al.  What the Eyes Perceive, The Brain Ignores: A Case of Pure Unilateral Representational Neglect , 1997, Cortex.

[43]  Robert K. Heaton,et al.  Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Manual – Revised and Expanded , 1993 .

[44]  P. Hoek The Stone in the Nest of the Swallow , 1880, Nature.

[45]  Peter Brugger,et al.  Random number generation in neglect patients reveals enhanced response stereotypy, but no neglect in number space , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[46]  Georg Kerkhoff,et al.  Spatial hemineglect in humans , 2001, Progress in Neurobiology.

[47]  Franco Cauda,et al.  Mental number line disruption in a right-neglect patient after a left-hemisphere stroke , 2009, Brain and Cognition.

[48]  Jamie I. D. Campbell Handbook of mathematical cognition , 2004 .

[49]  Michel Denis,et al.  Preserved visuo-spatial transformations in representational neglect , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[50]  John C. Marshall,et al.  When Right Goes Left: An Investigation of Line Bisection in a Case of Visual Neglect , 1989, Cortex.

[51]  J. Marshall,et al.  Spatial cognition: evidence from visual neglect , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[52]  Y. Rossetti,et al.  Pseudoneglect in schizophrenia: A line bisection study with cueing , 2007, Cognitive neuropsychiatry.

[53]  Marco Zorzi,et al.  Explicit versus Implicit Processing of Representational Space in Neglect: Dissociations in Accessing the Mental Number Line , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[54]  D. Boisson,et al.  Does Action Make the Link Between Number and Space Representation? , 2004, Psychological science.

[55]  H. Coslett Neglect in vision and visual imagery: a double dissociation. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[56]  Jean-Philippe van Dijck,et al.  A working memory account for spatial–numerical associations , 2011, Cognition.

[57]  M. Lezak Neuropsychological assessment, 3rd ed. , 1995 .

[58]  T. Schenkenberg,et al.  Line bisection and unilateral visual neglect in patients with neurologic impairment , 1980, Neurology.

[59]  D. C. Howell,et al.  Comparing an Individual's Test Score Against Norms Derived from Small Samples , 1998 .

[60]  Paul H. Garthwaite,et al.  Wanted: Fully Operational Definitions of Dissociations in Single-Case Studies , 2003, Cortex.

[61]  Y. Rossetti,et al.  Exaggerated leftward bias in the mental number line of patients with schizophrenia , 2007, Brain and Cognition.

[62]  FRANCIS GALTON,et al.  Visualised Numerals , 1880, Nature.

[63]  Masami Ishihara,et al.  Neglect "around the clock": Dissociating number and spatial neglect in right brain damage , 2011 .

[64]  F. Tomaiuolo,et al.  The anatomy of neglect without hemianopia: a key role for parietal–frontal disconnection? , 2003, Neuroreport.

[65]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Interactions between number and space in parietal cortex , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[66]  Marco Zorzi,et al.  The spatial representation of numbers: evidence from neglect and pseudoneglect , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[67]  M. Schlossberg The Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery: Theory and Clinical Interpretation. , 1986 .

[68]  D. Perani,et al.  The anatomy of unilateral neglect after right-hemisphere stroke lesions. A clinical/CT-scan correlation study in man , 1986, Neuropsychologia.