The relationship between paired associate learning and phonological skills in normally developing readers.

Seventy-five 6- to 11-year-old children were administered tests of phonological awareness, verbal short term memory (STM), and visual-verbal paired associate learning (PA learning) to investigate their relationship with word recognition and decoding skills. Phonological awareness was a stronger concurrent predictor of word recognition than verbal STM, and phonological awareness but not verbal STM was a predictor of learning in the PA learning task. Importantly, measures of phonological awareness and PA learning both accounted for independent variance in word reading, even when decoding skill was controlled. The results suggest that PA learning and phonological awareness tasks tap two separate mechanisms involved in learning to read. The results are discussed in relation to current theories of reading development.

[1]  Gordon D. A. Brown Connectionism, Phonology, Reading, and Regularity in Developmental Dyslexia , 1997, Brain and Language.

[2]  D. Share Phonological recoding and self-teaching: sine qua non of reading acquisition , 1995, Cognition.

[3]  Carsten Elbro,et al.  Early linguistic abilities and reading development: A review and a hypothesis , 1996 .

[4]  Paired associate learning in reading-disabled children: evidence for a rule-learning deficiency. , 1987, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[5]  Paul W. B. Atkins,et al.  Models of reading aloud: Dual-route and parallel-distributed-processing approaches. , 1993 .

[6]  Margaret J. Snowling,et al.  Graded Nonword Reading Test , 1996 .

[7]  Patricia A. McMullen,et al.  Converging methods for understanding reading and dyslexia , 1999 .

[8]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  Phonology, reading acquisition, and dyslexia: insights from connectionist models. , 1999 .

[9]  Franklin R. Manis,et al.  Acquisition of word identification skills in normal and disabled readers , 1985 .

[10]  C. Hulme,et al.  Segmentation, not rhyming, predicts early progress in learning to read. , 1997, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[11]  V. M. Holmes,et al.  Subtypes of developmental dyslexia and lexical acquisition , 1996 .

[12]  Marilyn Jager Adams,et al.  Beginning To Read: Thinking and Learning about Print. , 1991 .

[13]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Understanding normal and impaired word reading: computational principles in quasi-regular domains. , 1996, Psychological review.

[14]  C. Hulme,et al.  The effects of phonetic similarity and list length on children's sound categorization performance. , 1994, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[15]  James L. McClelland,et al.  A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. , 1989, Psychological review.

[16]  John Mc Leod,et al.  Some Psycholinguistic Correlates of Reading Disability in Young Children , 1967 .

[17]  H. Wimmer,et al.  Poor Reading: A Deficit in Skill-Automatization or a Phonological Deficit? , 1998 .

[18]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  See Dick RAN: Rapid Naming and the Longitudinal Prediction of Reading Subskills in First and Second Graders , 1999 .

[19]  Charles Hulme,et al.  Reading retardation and multi-sensory teaching , 1981 .

[20]  M. Wolf,et al.  The double-deficit hypothesis for the developmental dyslexias. , 1999 .

[21]  P. D. de Jong,et al.  Phonological sensitivity and the acquisition of new words in children. , 2000, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[22]  Brian Byrne,et al.  The learnability of the alphabetic principle: Children's initial hypotheses about how print represents spoken language , 1996, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[23]  A. Monk,et al.  Learning to read: the role of short-term memory and phonological skills. , 1994, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[24]  Keith E. Stanovich,et al.  Components of phonological awareness , 1995 .

[25]  U. Goswami,et al.  Picture Naming Deficits in Developmental Dyslexia: The Phonological Representations Hypothesis , 1997, Brain and Language.

[26]  H Wimmer,et al.  Pseudoname learning by German-speaking children with dyslexia: evidence for a phonological learning deficit. , 2000, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[27]  S. Brady,et al.  Vocabulary acquisition and reading ability , 1991 .

[28]  F. Vellutino,et al.  Semantic and phonological coding in poor and normal readers. , 1995, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[29]  M. Coltheart Lexical access in simple reading tasks , 1978 .

[30]  Charles Hulme,et al.  Deficits in output phonology: an explanation of reading failure? Cognit , 1992 .

[31]  Joseph K. Torgesen,et al.  Development of Reading-Related Phonological Processing Abilities: New Evidence of Bidirectional Causality from a Latent Variable Longitudinal Study. , 1994 .

[32]  Ingvar Lundberg,et al.  Reading and spelling skills in the first school years predicted from phonemic awareness skills in kindergarten , 1980 .

[33]  J M VANDERPLAS,et al.  The association value of random shapes. , 1959, Journal of experimental psychology.

[34]  Hallie Kay Yopp,et al.  The validity and reliability of phonemic awareness tests. , 1988 .

[35]  G. Underwood Strategies of information processing , 1980 .

[36]  F. Vellutino,et al.  Verbal vs non-verbal paired-associates learning in poor and normal readers , 1975, Neuropsychologia.