Additivity in prism adaptation as manifested in intermanual and interocular transfer

Level of adaptation was assessed in both exposed and unexposed eye and/or hand for visual shift (VS), proprioceptive shift (PS), and the eye-hand coordination, negative after effect (NA) measure of both visual and proprioceptive change, following 15-min and 20-diopter base-right displacement viewing of the active hand, under conditions of unconstrained head movement and terminal exposure feedback. Transfer was complete for the VS test, and significant, but incomplete for the PS and NA tests. For both exposed and unexposed eye/hand situations, level of adaptation was greater for the NA than for the PS test, which in turn showed greater adaptation than the VS test. Additivity was virtually perfect for the unexposed eye/hand (VS+PS = NA), but underadditivity appeared for the exposed eye/hand (VS+PS < NA). This underadditivity was approximately equal in magnitude to the amount that transfer on the NA test was less than on the PS test, suggesting that underadditivity was due to a nontransferable, assimilated corrective response in the NA test with the exposed eye/hand. Possible explanations for intermanual transfer are discussed.

[1]  Wilkinson Da,et al.  Visual-motor control loop: a linear system? , 1971 .

[2]  C. S. Harris,et al.  Beware of the Straight-Ahead Shift—A Nonperceptual Change in Experiments on Adaptation to Displaced Vision , 1974, Perception.

[3]  R B Welch,et al.  Evidence for a three-component model of prism adaptation. , 1974, Journal of experimental psychology.

[4]  Ian P. Howard,et al.  Additivity of components of prismatic adaptation , 1974 .

[5]  R B Welch,et al.  Research on Adaptation to Rearranged Vision: 1966–1974 , 1974, Perception.

[6]  G. M. Redding,et al.  Sources of “overadditivity” in prism adaptation , 1978, Perception & psychophysics.

[7]  C R HAMILTON,et al.  INTERMANUAL TRANSFER OF ADAPTATION TO PRISMS. , 1964, The American journal of psychology.

[8]  H. Pick,et al.  Visual and proprioceptive adaptation to optical displacement of the visual stimulus. , 1966, Journal of experimental psychology.

[9]  S. C. Mclaughlin,et al.  Changes in straight-ahead eye position during adaptation to wedge prisms , 1967 .

[10]  John L. Bower,et al.  Selective intermanual transfer of adaptive effects during adaptation to prism , 1965 .

[11]  B Wallace,et al.  Stability of Wilkinson's Linear Model of Prism Adaptation over Time for Various Targets , 1977, Perception.

[12]  M M Cohen,et al.  Changes in Auditory Localization following Prismatic Exposure under Continuous and Terminal Visual Feedback , 1974, Perceptual and motor skills.

[13]  B Wallace Visuomotor coordination and intermanual transfer for a proprioceptive reaching task. , 1978, Journal of motor behavior.

[14]  S M Ebenholtz,et al.  On the relation between interocular transfer of adaptation and Hering's law of equal innervation. , 1970, Psychological review.

[15]  R. Welch Perceptual modification : adapting to altered sensory environments / Robert B. Welch , 1978 .

[16]  G M Redding,et al.  Additivity in adaptation to optical tilt. , 1978, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[17]  I. Kohler,et al.  The formation and transformation of the perceptual world. , 1963 .

[18]  J E Foley,et al.  Interocular Effects in Prism Adaptation , 1969, Science.

[19]  R B Welch,et al.  Variables affecting the intermanual transfer and decay of prism adaptation. , 1974, Journal of experimental psychology.

[20]  Components of displacement adaptation in acquisition and decay as a function of hand and hall exposure , 1976 .