Comparing search patterns in digital breast tomosynthesis and full-field digital mammography: an eye tracking study

Abstract. As a promising imaging modality, digital breast tomosynthesis (DBT) leads to better diagnostic performance than traditional full-field digital mammograms (FFDM) alone. DBT allows different planes of the breast to be visualized, reducing occlusion from overlapping tissue. Although DBT is gaining popularity, best practices for search strategies in this medium are unclear. Eye tracking allowed us to describe search patterns adopted by radiologists searching DBT and FFDM images. Eleven radiologists examined eight DBT and FFDM cases. Observers marked suspicious masses with mouse clicks. Eye position was recorded at 1000 Hz and was coregistered with slice/depth plane as the radiologist scrolled through the DBT images, allowing a 3-D representation of eye position. Hit rate for masses was higher for tomography cases than 2-D cases and DBT led to lower false positive rates. However, search duration was much longer for DBT cases than FFDM. DBT was associated with longer fixations but similar saccadic amplitude compared with FFDM. When comparing radiologists’ eye movements to a previous study, which tracked eye movements as radiologists read chest CT, we found DBT viewers did not align with previously identified “driller” or “scanner” strategies, although their search strategy most closely aligns with a type of vigorous drilling strategy.

[1]  Andrew D. A. Maidment,et al.  Implementation of Synthesized Two-dimensional Mammography in a Population-based Digital Breast Tomosynthesis Screening Program. , 2016, Radiology.

[2]  K S Berbaum,et al.  Role of faulty visual search in the satisfaction of search effect in chest radiography. , 1998, Academic radiology.

[3]  Pragya A. Dang,et al.  Addition of tomosynthesis to conventional digital mammography: effect on image interpretation time of screening examinations. , 2014, Radiology.

[4]  Solveig Hofvind,et al.  Breast cancer: missed interval and screening-detected cancer at full-field digital mammography and screen-film mammography-- results from a retrospective review. , 2012, Radiology.

[5]  Trafton Drew,et al.  Scanners and drillers: characterizing expert visual search through volumetric images. , 2013, Journal of vision.

[6]  Andrea J Cook,et al.  Are radiologists' goals for mammography accuracy consistent with published recommendations? , 2012, Academic radiology.

[7]  Frans W Cornelissen,et al.  The Eyelink Toolbox: Eye tracking with MATLAB and the Psychophysics Toolbox , 2002, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[8]  D. Kopans,et al.  Digital Breast Tomosynthesis: State of the Art. , 2015, Radiology.

[9]  C. Mello-Thoms Perception of breast cancer: eye-position analysis of mammogram interpretation. , 2003, Academic radiology.

[10]  E. Conant,et al.  Holistic component of image perception in mammogram interpretation: gaze-tracking study. , 2007, Radiology.

[11]  Andriy I. Bandos,et al.  Comparison of digital mammography alone and digital mammography plus tomosynthesis in a population-based screening program. , 2013, Radiology.

[12]  L. Tabár,et al.  Swedish two-county trial: impact of mammographic screening on breast cancer mortality during 3 decades. , 2011, Radiology.

[13]  E. Conant,et al.  How experience and training influence mammography expertise. , 1999, Academic radiology.

[14]  Antje C Venjakob,et al.  Review of prospects and challenges of eye tracking in volumetric imaging , 2015, Journal of medical imaging.

[15]  Federica Zanca,et al.  Two-view and single-view tomosynthesis versus full-field digital mammography: high-resolution X-ray imaging observer study. , 2012, Radiology.

[16]  David Gur,et al.  Time to diagnosis and performance levels during repeat interpretations of digital breast tomosynthesis: preliminary observations. , 2010, Academic radiology.

[17]  Jules Sumkin,et al.  Effects of lesion conspicuity on visual search in mammogram reading. , 2005, Academic radiology.

[18]  Alasdair D. F. Clarke,et al.  People Are Unable to Recognize or Report on Their Own Eye Movements , 2017, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[19]  N Houssami,et al.  Application of breast tomosynthesis in screening: incremental effect on mammography acquisition and reading time. , 2012, The British journal of radiology.

[20]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[21]  Emily F Conant,et al.  Breast cancer screening using tomosynthesis in combination with digital mammography. , 2014, JAMA.

[22]  H L Kundel,et al.  Searching for lung nodules. A comparison of human performance with random and systematic scanning models. , 1987, Investigative radiology.

[23]  E. Krupinski,et al.  Comparison of eye position versus computer identified microcalcification clusters on mammograms. , 1997, Medical physics.

[24]  Katherine P Andriole,et al.  Optimizing analysis, visualization, and navigation of large image data sets: one 5000-section CT scan can ruin your whole day. , 2011, Radiology.

[25]  D. Miglioretti,et al.  Individual and Combined Effects of Age, Breast Density, and Hormone Replacement Therapy Use on the Accuracy of Screening Mammography , 2003, Annals of Internal Medicine.

[26]  D. Hansell,et al.  Thin-section CT of the lungs: eye-tracking analysis of the visual approach to reading tiled and stacked display formats. , 2006, European journal of radiology.

[27]  Zhengqiang Jiang,et al.  Analyzing visual-search observers using eye-tracking data for digital breast tomosynthesis images. , 2017, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[28]  David Gur,et al.  Digital breast tomosynthesis: observer performance study. , 2009, AJR. American journal of roentgenology.

[29]  Madhavi Raghu,et al.  Comparison of tomosynthesis plus digital mammography and digital mammography alone for breast cancer screening. , 2013, Radiology.

[30]  H L Kundel,et al.  Visual scanning, pattern recognition and decision-making in pulmonary nodule detection. , 1978, Investigative radiology.

[31]  Marcus Nyström,et al.  Investigation of viewing procedures for interpretation of breast tomosynthesis image volumes: a detection-task study with eye tracking , 2012, European Radiology.

[32]  Jeremy M Wolfe,et al.  You think you know where you looked? You better look again. , 2016, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.