Motor Reproduction of Time Interval Depends on Internal Temporal Cues in the Brain: Sensorimotor Imagery in Rhythm

How the human brain perceives time intervals is a fascinating topic that has been explored in many fields of study. This study examined how time intervals are replicated in three conditions: with no internalized cue (PT), with an internalized cue without a beat (AS), and with an internalized cue with a beat (RS). In PT, participants accurately reproduced the time intervals up to approximately 3 s. Over 3 s, however, the reproduction errors became increasingly negative. In RS, longer presentations of over 5.6 s and 13 beats induced accurate time intervals in reproductions. This suggests longer exposure to beat presentation leads to stable internalization and efficiency in the sensorimotor processing of perception and reproduction. In AS, up to approximately 3 s, the results were similar to those of RS whereas over 3 s, the results shifted and became similar to those of PT. The time intervals between the first two stimuli indicate that the strategies of time-interval reproduction in AS may shift from RS to PT. Neural basis underlying the reproduction of time intervals without a beat may depend on length of time interval between adjacent stimuli in sequences.

[1]  M. Yumoto,et al.  Implicit and explicit statistical learning of tone sequences across spectral shifts , 2014, Neuropsychologia.

[2]  C. Wright,et al.  Temporal rescaling of simple and complex ratios in rhythmic tapping. , 1995 .

[3]  Ernst Pöppel,et al.  Temporal processing deficits in high-functioning children with autism. , 2004, British journal of psychology.

[4]  M. Yumoto,et al.  Statistical learning of an auditory sequence and reorganization of acquired knowledge: A time course of word segmentation and ordering , 2017, Neuropsychologia.

[5]  M. Wittmann,et al.  Hemispheric specialisation for self-paced motor sequences. , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[6]  Temporal integration in a subjective accentuation task as a function of child cognitive development , 1998, Neuroscience Letters.

[7]  Akira Toyomura,et al.  Effect of an 8-week practice of externally triggered speech on basal ganglia activity of stuttering and fluent speakers , 2015, NeuroImage.

[8]  Ernst Pöppel,et al.  Pre-semantically defined temporal windows for cognitive processing , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[9]  F. Vidal,et al.  Functional Anatomy of the Attentional Modulation of Time Estimation , 2004, Science.

[10]  Ernst Pöppel,et al.  Temporal processing disorders in patients with Broca's aphasia , 1997, Neuroscience Letters.

[11]  D. V. von Cramon,et al.  Interval and ordinal properties of sequences are associated with distinct premotor areas. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[12]  D. Poeppel,et al.  Temporal window of integration in auditory-visual speech perception , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[13]  P. Fraisse Perception and estimation of time. , 1984, Annual review of psychology.

[14]  E. Pöppel,et al.  A hierarchical model of temporal perception , 1997, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[15]  Temporal Integration of the Brain as Studied with the Metronome Paradigm , 1997 .

[16]  M. Bozkurt,et al.  Functional anatomy. , 1980, Equine veterinary journal.

[17]  Yoshihiro Miyake,et al.  Two types of anticipation in synchronization tapping. , 2004, Acta neurobiologiae experimentalis.

[18]  M. Yumoto,et al.  Pitch-class distribution modulates the statistical learning of atonal chord sequences , 2016, Brain and Cognition.

[19]  Catalin V. Buhusi,et al.  What makes us tick? Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[20]  E Pöppel,et al.  Oscillations as possible basis for time perception. , 1971, Studium generale; Zeitschrift fur die Einheit der Wissenschaften im Zusammenhang ihrer Begriffsbildungen und Forschungsmethoden.

[21]  B. Ross,et al.  Endogenous Neuromagnetic Activity for Mental Hierarchy of Timing , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[22]  Bruno H. Repp,et al.  Hearing a melody in different ways: Multistability of metrical interpretation, reflected in rate limits of sensorimotor synchronization , 2007, Cognition.

[23]  M. Linn,et al.  Emergence and characterization of sex differences in spatial ability: a meta-analysis. , 1985, Child development.

[24]  M. Peters The relationship between variability of intertap intervals and interval duration , 1989 .

[25]  S. Samson,et al.  Is time reproduction sensitive to sensory modalities? , 2009 .

[26]  T Elbert,et al.  The processing of temporal intervals reflected by CNV-like brain potentials. , 1991, Psychophysiology.

[27]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  The Functional Neuroanatomy of Temporal Discrimination , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[28]  J G Martin,et al.  Rhythmic (hierarchical) versus serial structure in speech and other behavior. , 1972, Psychological review.

[29]  D. Buonomano,et al.  The neural basis of temporal processing. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.

[30]  C. Krumhansl,et al.  Mental representations for musical meter. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[31]  Riikka Möttönen,et al.  Auditory-Motor Processing of Speech Sounds , 2012, Cerebral cortex.

[32]  Elzbieta Szelag,et al.  Cortical involvement in temporal reproduction: evidence for differential roles of the hemispheres , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[33]  Masato Yumoto,et al.  Single, but not dual, attention facilitates statistical learning of two concurrent auditory sequences , 2017, Scientific Reports.

[34]  T. Tao,et al.  A VARIATION , 2009 .

[35]  W. Matthews How does sequence structure affect the judgment of time? Exploring a weighted sum of segments model , 2013, Cognitive Psychology.

[36]  E. Pöppel,et al.  Temporal order judgement for auditory and visual stimuli. , 2002, Acta neurobiologiae experimentalis.

[37]  Barbara Tillmann,et al.  Rhythmic auditory stimulation influences syntactic processing in children with developmental language disorders. , 2013, Neuropsychology.

[38]  Stephen Handel,et al.  The contextual nature of rhythmic interpretation , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.

[39]  M. Wittmann,et al.  Temporal reproduction: further evidence for two processes. , 2007, Acta psychologica.

[40]  Massimiliano Di Luca,et al.  Taking a long look at isochrony: Perceived duration increases with temporal, but not stimulus regularity , 2014, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.

[41]  Maria Wimber,et al.  Perceived time and temporal structure: Neural entrainment to isochronous stimulation increases duration estimates , 2016, NeuroImage.

[42]  R. Miall,et al.  Distinct systems for automatic and cognitively controlled time measurement: evidence from neuroimaging , 2003, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[43]  M. Thaut,et al.  Neurobiological foundations of neurologic music therapy: rhythmic entrainment and the motor system , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[44]  P J Essens,et al.  Hierarchical organization of temporal patterns , 1986, Perception & psychophysics.

[45]  Tatsuya Daikoku,et al.  Auditory Statistical Learning During Concurrent Physical Exercise and the Tolerance for Pitch, Tempo, and Rhythm Changes. , 2017, Motor control.

[46]  G. Madison,et al.  Variability in isochronous tapping: higher order dependencies as a function of intertap interval. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[47]  S. Tobimatsu,et al.  Spatiotemporal brain dynamics of auditory temporal assimilation , 2017, Scientific Reports.

[48]  M. Wittmann The inner experience of time , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[49]  I. Hirsh,et al.  Perceived order in different sense modalities. , 1961, Journal of experimental psychology.

[50]  T. Daikoku Neurophysiological Markers of Statistical Learning in Music and Language: Hierarchy, Entropy and Uncertainty , 2018, Brain sciences.

[51]  Elzbieta Szelag,et al.  Individual differences in temporal information processing in humans. , 2004, Acta neurobiologiae experimentalis.

[52]  M. Jones,et al.  Temporal Aspects of Stimulus-Driven Attending in Dynamic Arrays , 2002, Psychological science.

[53]  J. Tanji,et al.  Interval time coding by neurons in the presupplementary and supplementary motor areas , 2009, Nature Neuroscience.

[54]  Stefan Koelsch,et al.  Under the hood of statistical learning: A statistical MMN reflects the magnitude of transitional probabilities in auditory sequences , 2016, Scientific Reports.

[55]  B. Ross,et al.  Beta-Band Oscillations Represent Auditory Beat and Its Metrical Hierarchy in Perception and Imagery , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[56]  Matthew Brett,et al.  Rhythm and Beat Perception in Motor Areas of the Brain , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[57]  O Hikosaka,et al.  Neural Representation of a Rhythm Depends on Its Interval Ratio , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[58]  E. Szelag,et al.  Reproduction of auditory and visual standards in monochannel cochlear implant users. , 2004, Acta neurobiologiae experimentalis.

[59]  G Aschersleben,et al.  Synchronizing actions with events: The role of sensory information , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[60]  E Pöppel,et al.  Temporal constraints in processing of nonverbal rhythmic patterns. , 1996, Acta neurobiologiae experimentalis.

[61]  B. Ross,et al.  Internalized Timing of Isochronous Sounds Is Represented in Neuromagnetic Beta Oscillations , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[62]  Jacques Paillard,et al.  III. - Quelques donnes psychophysiologiques relatives au dclenchement de la commande motrice , 1946 .

[63]  Tatsuya Daikoku,et al.  Physical fitness modulates incidental but not intentional statistical learning of simultaneous auditory sequences during concurrent physical exercise , 2017, Neurological research.

[64]  Ernst Pöppel,et al.  Duration processing in children as determined by time reproduction: implications for a few seconds temporal window. , 2002, Acta psychologica.

[65]  R. Dean,et al.  Evidence for multiple strategies in off-beat tapping with anisochronous stimuli , 2014, Psychological research.

[66]  R. Zatorre,et al.  Listening to musical rhythms recruits motor regions of the brain. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[67]  R. Schaefer,et al.  Decomposing rhythm processing: electroencephalography of perceived and self-imposed rhythmic patterns , 2010, Psychological research.

[68]  Ernst Pöppel,et al.  Temporal mechanisms of the brain as fundamentals of communication — with special reference to music perception and performance , 1999 .

[69]  M. R. Jones,et al.  Dynamic attending and responses to time. , 1989, Psychological review.

[70]  T Radil,et al.  Cooperative tapping: Time control under different feedback conditions , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[71]  J. Churan,et al.  Stimulus-dependent processing of temporal order , 2006, Behavioural Processes.

[72]  Steven L. Miller,et al.  Temporal Processing Deficits of Language-Learning Impaired Children Ameliorated by Training , 1996, Science.

[73]  Masato Yumoto,et al.  Statistical learning of music- and language-like sequences and tolerance for spectral shifts , 2015, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[74]  Riikka Möttönen,et al.  Attention Fine-Tunes Auditory–Motor Processing of Speech Sounds , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[75]  Knight Dunlap,et al.  Reaction to rhythmic stimuli with attempt to synchronize. , 1910 .

[76]  Sigmund Exner,et al.  Experimentelle Untersuchung der einfachsten psychischen Processe , 1875, Archiv für die gesamte Physiologie des Menschen und der Tiere.

[77]  W. Meck,et al.  Neuropsychological mechanisms of interval timing behavior. , 2000, BioEssays : news and reviews in molecular, cellular and developmental biology.

[78]  J. Mates,et al.  Temporal Integration in Sensorimotor Synchronization , 1994, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[79]  David Poeppel,et al.  Visual speech speeds up the neural processing of auditory speech. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[80]  Tatsuya Daikoku,et al.  The variation of hemodynamics relative to listening to consonance or dissonance during chord progression , 2012, Neurological research.

[81]  Temporal Integration. , 1958, Canadian Medical Association journal.