Mental models change rapidly with implicitly acquired information about the local environment: a two-tone image study.

The projection of 3-D objects to 2-D images necessitates a loss of information, thus the shape of volumetric objects depicted in images is inherently ambiguous. The results of 3 experiments suggest observers use mental models of the local visual environment to constrain image interpretation. These models change quickly and dramatically to accommodate implicitly acquired information. Observers viewed very high-contrast (2-tone) images of novel volumetric objects. Before priming, novel 2-tone images appeared 2-D. After incidental exposure to similar objects in grayscale or familiar objects in 2-tone, the test images appeared volumetric. Incidental leaming appears to alter observers' mental models, thus causing an alteration in image interpretation in the absence of any image change. Highlights were interpreted more accurately than shadows, suggesting shadows play a secondary role in shape recovery.

[1]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Shape from shadows. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[2]  Edward H. Adelson,et al.  The perception of shading and reflectance , 1996 .

[3]  Tomaso Poggio,et al.  Computational vision and regularization theory , 1985, Nature.

[4]  W. Mauch,et al.  Contents , 2007, European Neuropsychopharmacology.

[5]  V. Ramachandran,et al.  On the perception of shape from shading , 1988, Nature.

[6]  G. Sperling A Model for Visual Memory Tasks1 , 1963, Human factors.

[7]  M. Lindauer,et al.  A Psychology of Picture Perception: Images and Information , 1974 .

[8]  John M. Kennedy,et al.  Optics Painting and Photography , 1970 .

[9]  I. Biederman,et al.  Searching for objects in real-world scences. , 1973, Journal of experimental psychology.

[10]  Michael K. McBeath,et al.  Perceptual Bias for Forward-Facing Motion , 1992 .

[11]  R. Gregory Eye and Brain: The Psychology of Seeing , 1966 .

[12]  D. Knill,et al.  Apparent surface curvature affects lightness perception , 1991, Nature.

[13]  Patrick Henry Winston,et al.  The psychology of computer vision , 1976, Pattern Recognit..

[14]  R. Gregory Perceptions as hypotheses. , 1980, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[15]  Alan L. Yuille,et al.  Perception as Bayesian Inference: Introduction , 1996 .

[16]  A. Pentland Finding the illuminant direction , 1982 .

[17]  James T. Todd,et al.  Ordinal structure in the visual perception and cognition of smoothly curved surfaces. , 1989 .

[18]  D. Knill,et al.  Implications of a Bayesian formulation of visual information for processing for psychophysics , 1996 .

[19]  I Rock,et al.  The Construction of a Plane from Pictorial Information , 1982, Perception.

[20]  E. Adelson Perceptual organization and the judgment of brightness. , 1993, Science.

[21]  Irvin Rock,et al.  Orientation and form , 1974 .

[22]  J. Hochberg Chapter 9 – Gestalt Theory and Its Legacy: Organization in Eye and Brain, in Attention and Mental Representation , 1998 .

[23]  J T Todd,et al.  Perception of rigid motion in depth from the optical deformations of shadows and occlusion boundaries. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[24]  S. Siegel,et al.  Nonparametric Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences , 2022, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.

[25]  D Kersten,et al.  Moving Cast Shadows Induce Apparent Motion in Depth , 1997, Perception.

[26]  David C. Knill,et al.  Introduction: a Bayesian formulation of visual perception , 1996 .

[27]  J. Gibson The perception of the visual world , 1951 .

[28]  C. M. Mooney Closure as affected by viewing time and multiple visual fixations. , 1957, Canadian journal of psychology.

[29]  N. Tinbergen,et al.  The Study of Instinct , 1953 .

[30]  David L. Waltz,et al.  Understanding Line drawings of Scenes with Shadows , 1975 .

[31]  Pascal Mamassian,et al.  Observer biases in the 3D interpretation of line drawings , 1998, Vision Research.

[32]  Kent A. Stevens,et al.  The Visual Interpretation of Surface Contours , 1981, Artif. Intell..

[33]  William T. Freeman,et al.  The generic viewpoint assumption in a framework for visual perception , 1994, Nature.

[34]  tephen E. Palmer The effects of contextual scenes on the identification of objects , 1975, Memory & cognition.

[35]  Pawel Lewicki,et al.  Acquisition of procedural knowledge about a pattern of stimuli that cannot be articulated , 1988, Cognitive Psychology.

[36]  J Sun,et al.  Shading and Stereo in Early Perception of Shape and Reflectance , 1997, Perception.

[37]  Pietro Perona,et al.  Early computation of shape and reflectance in the visual system , 1996, Nature.

[38]  Harry G. Barrow,et al.  Interpreting Line Drawings as Three-Dimensional Surfaces , 1980, Artif. Intell..

[39]  Neil A. Macmillan,et al.  Detection Theory: A User's Guide , 1991 .

[40]  Donald D. Hoffman,et al.  Parts of recognition , 1984, Cognition.

[41]  E. Land,et al.  Lightness and retinex theory. , 1971, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[42]  Ronald A. Rensink,et al.  Influence of scene-based properties on visual search. , 1990, Science.

[43]  K. Rayner,et al.  Is visual information integrated across saccades? , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.

[44]  Diane J. Schiano,et al.  Tops are more salient than bottoms , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[45]  A. Reber Implicit learning and tacit knowledge , 1993 .

[46]  K Nakayama,et al.  Experiencing and perceiving visual surfaces. , 1992, Science.

[47]  E. Brunswik Perception and the Representative Design of Psychological Experiments , 1957 .

[48]  M. Baxandall Shadows and Enlightenment , 1995 .

[49]  G. Logan Repetition priming and automaticity: Common underlying mechanisms? , 1990, Cognitive Psychology.

[50]  I. Biederman,et al.  Size invariance in visual object priming , 1992 .

[51]  I. Biederman,et al.  Recognizing depth-rotated objects: Evidence and conditions for three-dimensional viewpoint invariance. , 1993 .

[52]  M. Chun,et al.  Contextual Cueing: Implicit Learning and Memory of Visual Context Guides Spatial Attention , 1998, Cognitive Psychology.

[53]  I. Rock The Logic of Perception , 1983 .

[54]  A. Witkin,et al.  On the Role of Structure in Vision , 1983 .

[55]  J. O'Regan,et al.  Solving the "real" mysteries of visual perception: the world as an outside memory. , 1992, Canadian journal of psychology.

[56]  V. S. Ramachandran,et al.  Perception of shape from shading , 1988, Nature.

[57]  Patrick Cavanagh,et al.  Recovery of 3D volume from 2-tone images of novel objects , 1998, Cognition.

[58]  Heinrich H. Bülthoff,et al.  Object recognition in man, monkey, and machine , 1999 .

[59]  K. Berbaum,et al.  Light Source Position in the Perception of Object Shape , 1983, Perception.

[60]  A. Yonas,et al.  The role of frames of reference in the development of responsiveness to shading information. , 1979, Child development.

[61]  J. Braun Shape-from-shading is independent of visual attention and may be a 'texton'. , 1993, Spatial vision.

[62]  David Marr,et al.  VISION A Computational Investigation into the Human Representation and Processing of Visual Information , 2009 .

[63]  I. Rock,et al.  Pictorial Depth and Related Constancy Effects as a Function of Recognition , 1978, Perception.

[64]  Julian Hochberg,et al.  Perception and cognition at century's end , 1998 .

[65]  Jan J. Koenderink,et al.  Solid shape , 1990 .

[66]  Azriel Rosenfeld,et al.  Human and Machine Vision , 1983 .

[67]  Julian Hochberg,et al.  The Affordances of Perceptual Inquiry: Pictures are Learned from the World, and What that Fact Might Mean About Perception Quite Generally , 1997 .

[68]  Stillso n The Psychology of Vision , 1932 .

[69]  Tomaso Poggio,et al.  Role of learning in three-dimensional form perception , 1996, Nature.