Statistical Learning Signals in Macaque Inferior Temporal Cortex

Humans are sensitive to statistical regularities in their visual environment, but the nature of the underlying neural statistical learning signals still remains to be clarified. As in human behavioral and neuroimaging studies of statistical learning, we exposed rhesus monkeys to a continuous stream of images, presented without interstimulus interval or reward association. The stimulus set consisted of 3 groups of 5 images each (quintets). The stimulus order within each quintet was fixed, but the quintets were presented repeatedly in a random order without interruption. Thus, only transitional probabilities defined quintets of images. Postexposure recordings in inferior temporal (IT) cortex showed an enhanced response to stimuli that violated the exposed sequence. This enhancement was found only for stimuli that were not predicted by the just preceding stimulus, reflecting a temporally adjacent stimulus relationship, and was sensitive to stimulus order. By comparing IT responses with sequences with and without statistical regularities, we observed a short latency, transient response suppression for stimuli of the sequence with regularities, in addition to a later sustained response enhancement to stimuli that violated the sequence with regularities. These findings constrain models of mechanisms underlying neural responses in predictable temporal sequences, such as predictive coding.

[1]  Dwight J. Kravitz,et al.  The ventral visual pathway: an expanded neural framework for the processing of object quality , 2013, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[2]  Florent Meyniel,et al.  The Neural Representation of Sequences: From Transition Probabilities to Algebraic Patterns and Linguistic Trees , 2015, Neuron.

[3]  Yukiko Kikuchi,et al.  Auditory Artificial Grammar Learning in Macaque and Marmoset Monkeys , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[4]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A theory of cortical responses , 2005, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[5]  Yuka Sasaki,et al.  Different Dynamics of Performance and Brain Activation in the Time Course of Perceptual Learning , 2008, Neuron.

[6]  J. Saiki,et al.  Gift from statistical learning: Visual statistical learning enhances memory for sequence elements and impairs memory for items that disrupt regularities , 2016, Cognition.

[7]  R. Aslin,et al.  PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article UNSUPERVISED STATISTICAL LEARNING OF HIGHER-ORDER SPATIAL STRUCTURES FROM VISUAL SCENES , 2022 .

[8]  D. V. van Essen,et al.  The Processing of Visual Shape in the Cerebral Cortex of Human and Nonhuman Primates: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[9]  Lila Davachi,et al.  How the hippocampus preserves order: the role of prediction and context , 2015, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[10]  Yevgeniy B. Sirotin,et al.  Anticipatory haemodynamic signals in sensory cortex not predicted by local neuronal activity. , 2009, Nature.

[11]  Y. Benjamini,et al.  Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing , 1995 .

[12]  J. DiCarlo,et al.  Neuronal Learning of Invariant Object Representation in the Ventral Visual Stream Is Not Dependent on Reward , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[13]  A. Todorović,et al.  Repetition Suppression and Expectation Suppression Are Dissociable in Time in Early Auditory Evoked Fields , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  Marvin M. Chun,et al.  Neural Evidence of Statistical Learning: Efficient Detection of Visual Regularities Without Awareness , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[15]  J. DiCarlo,et al.  Unsupervised Natural Visual Experience Rapidly Reshapes Size-Invariant Object Representation in Inferior Temporal Cortex , 2010, Neuron.

[16]  R. Desimone,et al.  A neural mechanism for working and recognition memory in inferior temporal cortex. , 1991, Science.

[17]  Aaron R Seitz,et al.  Dissociable behavioural outcomes of visual statistical learning , 2015, Visual cognition.

[18]  M. Goldsmith,et al.  Statistical Learning by 8-Month-Old Infants , 1996 .

[19]  Scott P. Johnson,et al.  Statistical Learning Across Development: Flexible Yet Constrained , 2013, Front. Psychology.

[20]  Michael A. Arbib,et al.  Complex temporal sequence learning based on short-term memory , 1990 .

[21]  C. Olson,et al.  Repetition suppression in monkey inferotemporal cortex: relation to behavioral priming. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  Christopher J. Aura,et al.  Divergence of fMRI and neural signals in V1 during perceptual suppression in the awake monkey , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[23]  C. Olson,et al.  Statistical learning of visual transitions in monkey inferotemporal cortex , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[24]  E. Miller,et al.  Experience-dependent sharpening of visual shape selectivity in inferior temporal cortex. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[25]  David L. Sheinberg,et al.  Effects of Long-Term Visual Experience on Responses of Distinct Classes of Single Units in Inferior Temporal Cortex , 2012, Neuron.

[26]  Zachary P. Kilpatrick,et al.  Networks that learn the precise timing of event sequences , 2014, bioRxiv.

[27]  Michael D. Dodd,et al.  The influence of attention, learning, and motivation on visual search. , 2012, Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation.

[28]  Mark F. Bear,et al.  Learned spatiotemporal sequence recognition and prediction in primary visual cortex , 2014, Nature Neuroscience.

[29]  Keiji Tanaka,et al.  Inferotemporal cortex and object vision. , 1996, Annual review of neuroscience.

[30]  A. Borst Seeing smells: imaging olfactory learning in bees , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[31]  R. Gómez Variability and Detection of Invariant Structure , 2002, Psychological science.

[32]  R. Vogels,et al.  Neurons in Macaque Inferior Temporal Cortex Show No Surprise Response to Deviants in Visual Oddball Sequences , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[33]  R. Vogels,et al.  Effects of perceptual learning in visual backward masking on the responses of macaque inferior temporal neurons , 2007, Neuroscience.

[34]  Kenny Smith,et al.  Mixed‐complexity artificial grammar learning in humans and macaque monkeys: evaluating learning strategies , 2015, The European journal of neuroscience.

[35]  Kristin E. Sparks,et al.  Learning at a distance in south dakota: , 2001 .

[36]  R. Vogels,et al.  Properties of shape tuning of macaque inferior temporal neurons examined using rapid serial visual presentation. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[37]  E. Newport,et al.  Learning at a distance I. Statistical learning of non-adjacent dependencies , 2004, Cognitive Psychology.

[38]  N. Logothetis The Underpinnings of the BOLD Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Signal , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[39]  B. Rossion,et al.  Revisiting Snodgrass and Vanderwart's Object Pictorial Set: The Role of Surface Detail in Basic-Level Object Recognition , 2004, Perception.

[40]  M. Behrmann,et al.  Impact of learning on representation of parts and wholes in monkey inferotemporal cortex , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[41]  Y. Miyashita,et al.  Neural organization for the long-term memory of paired associates , 1991, Nature.

[42]  Yevgeniy B. Sirotin,et al.  The neuroimaging signal is a linear sum of neurally distinct stimulus- and task-related components , 2012, Nature Neuroscience.

[43]  G. Orban,et al.  Selectivity of Neuronal Adaptation Does Not Match Response Selectivity: A Single-Cell Study of the fMRI Adaptation Paradigm , 2006, Neuron.

[44]  W. Tecumseh Fitch,et al.  Action at a distance: dependency sensitivity in a New World primate , 2013, Biology Letters.

[45]  C. Summerfield,et al.  Expectation in perceptual decision making: neural and computational mechanisms , 2014, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[46]  Y. Miyashita,et al.  Configurational encoding of complex visual forms by single neurons of monkey temporal cortex , 1993, Neuropsychologia.

[47]  P. Perruchet,et al.  Implicit learning and statistical learning: one phenomenon, two approaches , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[48]  Marcia K. Johnson,et al.  Implicit Perceptual Anticipation Triggered by Statistical Learning , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[49]  S. Gerber,et al.  Unsupervised Natural Experience Rapidly Alters Invariant Object Representation in Visual Cortex , 2008 .

[50]  Carl R Olson,et al.  Statistical Learning of Serial Visual Transitions by Neurons in Monkey Inferotemporal Cortex , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[51]  Nicholas B Turk-Browne,et al.  Statistical learning and its consequences. , 2012, Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation.

[52]  C. Olson,et al.  Prediction suppression in monkey inferotemporal cortex depends on the conditional probability between images. , 2016, Journal of neurophysiology.

[53]  K. I. Blum,et al.  Functional significance of long-term potentiation for sequence learning and prediction. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[54]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Representation of Numerical and Sequential Patterns in Macaque and Human Brains , 2015, Current Biology.

[55]  R. Desimone,et al.  Responses of Macaque Perirhinal Neurons during and after Visual Stimulus Association Learning , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[56]  Jonathan Grainger,et al.  References and Notes , 2022 .

[57]  Y. Miyashita Neuronal correlate of visual associative long-term memory in the primate temporal cortex , 1988, Nature.

[58]  R. Aslin,et al.  Statistical learning of higher-order temporal structure from visual shape sequences. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[59]  Barbara Landau,et al.  The Necessity of the Medial Temporal Lobe for Statistical Learning , 2014, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[60]  Keiji Tanaka,et al.  Neuronal selectivities to complex object features in the ventral visual pathway of the macaque cerebral cortex. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[61]  M. Masson,et al.  Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.