Lexical and semantic binding in verbal short-term memory

Semantic dementia patients make numerous phoneme migration errors in their immediate serial recall of poorly comprehended words. In this study, similar errors were induced in the word recall of healthy participants by presenting unpredictable mixed lists of words and nonwords. This technique revealed that lexicality, word frequency, imageability, and the ratio of words to nonwords all influence the stability of the phonological trace. These factors affected phoneme migrations and phoneme identity errors for both the words themselves and the nonwords they were presented with. Therefore, lexical/semantic knowledge encourages the phonological segments of familiar words to emerge together in immediate serial recall. In the absence of such knowledge, the elements of a particular item are more likely to recombine with the phonemes of other list items. These findings demonstrate the importance of lexical and semantic binding in verbal short-term memory.

[1]  G. Humphreys,et al.  The Role of Semantic Knowledge in Short-term Memory , 2002, Neurocase.

[2]  D. Norris,et al.  The primacy model: a new model of immediate serial recall. , 1998, Psychological review.

[3]  Gordon D. A. Brown,et al.  Memory for familiar and unfamiliar words: Evidence for a long-term memory contribution to short-term memory span , 1991 .

[4]  M. Poirier,et al.  Immediate serial recall, word frequency, item identity and item position. , 1996, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[5]  J. Hodges,et al.  The Impact of Semantic Memory Loss on Phonological Representations , 1994, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[6]  V. Coltheart Effects of phonological similarity and concurrent irrelevant articulation on short-term-memory recall of repeated and novel word lists , 1993, Memory & cognition.

[7]  C. Hulme,et al.  The effects of word co-occurrence on short-term memory: associative links in long-term memory affect short-term memory performance. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[8]  N. Burgess Memory for Serial Order : A Network Model of the Phonological Loop and its Timing , 1999 .

[9]  Karalyn Patterson,et al.  Lexical and Semantic Binding Effects in Short-term Memory: Evidence from Semantic Dementia , 1997 .

[10]  Vernon H. Gregg,et al.  Word frequency, articulatory suppression and memory span , 1989 .

[11]  S. Belleville,et al.  How Loss of Meaning with Preservation of Phonological Word Form Affects Immediate Serial Recall Performance: A Linguistic Account , 2002, Neurocase.

[12]  D. Besner,et al.  Beyond the articulatory loop: A semantic contribution to serial order recall of subspan lists , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[13]  J. S. Nairne A feature model of immediate memory , 1990, Memory & cognition.

[14]  Susan E. Gathercole,et al.  Models Of Short-Term Memory , 1996 .

[15]  M. Poirier,et al.  Semantic Similarity and Immediate Serial Recall: Is There a Detrimental Effect on Order Information? , 1999, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[16]  R. Schweickert A multinomial processing tree model for degradation and redintegration in immediate recall , 1993, Memory & cognition.

[17]  M. Poirier,et al.  Memory for Related and Unrelated Words: Further Evidence on the Influence of Semantic Factors in Immediate Serial Recall , 1995, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[18]  R. Conrad,et al.  Acoustic confusions in immediate memory. , 1964 .

[19]  S. Pickering,et al.  Dissociable Lexical and Phonological Influences on Serial Recognition and Serial Recall , 2001, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[20]  Michael J. Watkins,et al.  Serial recall and the modality effect: Effects of word frequency. , 1977 .

[21]  G D Brown,et al.  Oscillator-based memory for serial order. , 2000, Psychological review.

[22]  Marie Poirier,et al.  Immediate serial recall of words and nonwords: Tests of the retrieval-based hypothesis , 2000, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[23]  D. Norris,et al.  THE QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY, 1996, 49A (1), 80 ± 115 Unchained Memory: Error Patterns Rule out Chaining Models of Immediate Serial Recall , 2022 .

[24]  R Treiman,et al.  Short-term memory errors for spoken syllables are affected by the linguistic structure of the syllables. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[25]  Michael Wilson MRC Psycholinguistic Database , 2001 .

[26]  M. D’Esposito Working memory. , 2008, Handbook of clinical neurology.

[27]  G D Brown,et al.  Word-frequency effects on short-term memory tasks: evidence for a redintegration process in immediate serial recall. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[28]  J. S. Nairne,et al.  A framework for interpreting recency effects in immediate serial recall , 1988, Memory & cognition.

[29]  Charles Hulme,et al.  Concrete words are easier to recall than abstract words: Evidence for a semantic contribution to short-term serial recall. , 1999 .

[30]  S. Pickering,et al.  Verbal and visuospatial short-term memory in children: Evidence for common and distinct mechanisms , 1998, Memory & cognition.

[31]  James L. McClelland,et al.  A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. , 1989, Psychological review.

[32]  S. Roodenrys,et al.  The effects of stimulus set size and word frequency on verbal serial recall , 2000, Memory.

[33]  Elizabeth Jefferies,et al.  Lexical and semantic influences on item and order memory in immediate serial recognition: Evidence from a novel task , 2006, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[34]  A. J. Hull,et al.  INFORMATION, ACOUSTIC CONFUSION AND MEMORY SPAN. , 1964, British journal of psychology.

[35]  G. Dell,et al.  Stages of lexical access in language production , 1992, Cognition.

[36]  R. H. Baayen,et al.  The CELEX Lexical Database (CD-ROM) , 1996 .

[37]  James L. McClelland,et al.  The TRACE model of speech perception , 1986, Cognitive Psychology.

[38]  J. Hodges,et al.  The role of speech production in auditory-verbal short-term memory: evidence from progressive fluent aphasia , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[39]  R. Brener,et al.  An experimental investigation of memory span , 1940 .

[40]  Nadine Martin Eleanor M. Saffran Language and Auditory-verbal Short-term Memory Impairments: Evidence for Common Underlying Processes , 1997 .

[41]  Susan E. Gathercole,et al.  Interactive processes in phonological memory , 1996 .

[42]  Matthew A. Lambon Ralph,et al.  A semantic contribution to nonword recall? Evidence for intact phonological processes in semantic dementia , 2005, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[43]  Roy W Jones,et al.  When does word meaning affect immediate serial recall in semantic dementia? , 2004, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[44]  A. Baddeley,et al.  The phonological loop as a language learning device. , 1998, Psychological review.

[45]  R. Henson Short-Term Memory for Serial Order: The Start-End Model , 1998, Cognitive Psychology.

[46]  Charles Hulme,et al.  High- and low-frequency words are recalled equally well in alternating lists: Evidence for associative effects in serial recall , 2003 .