A One-to-One Bias and Fast Mapping Support Preschoolers' Learning About Faces and Voices

A multimodal person representation contains information about what a person looks like and what a person sounds like. However, little is known about how children form these face-voice mappings. Here, we explored the possibility that two cognitive tools that guide word learning, a one-to-one mapping bias and fast mapping, also guide children's learning about faces and voices. We taught 4- and 5-year-olds mappings between three individual faces and voices, then presented them with new faces and voices. In Experiment 1, we found that children rapidly learned face-voice mappings from just a few exposures, and furthermore spontaneously mapped novel faces to novel voices using a one-to-one mapping bias (that each face can produce only one voice). In Experiment 2, we found that children's face-voice representations are abstract, generalizing to novel tokens of a person. In Experiment 3, we found that children retained in memory the face-voice mappings that they had generated via inference (i.e., they showed evidence of fast mapping), and used these newly formed representations to generate further mappings between new faces and voices. These findings suggest that preschoolers' rapid learning about faces and voices may be aided by biases that are similar to those that support word learning.

[1]  E. Markman,et al.  Word learning in children: an examination of fast mapping. , 1987, Child development.

[2]  J. Halberda,et al.  The development of a word-learning strategy , 2003, Cognition.

[3]  L. Bahrick,et al.  Intermodal perception of adult and child faces and voices by infants. , 1998, Child development.

[4]  E. Markman,et al.  Use of the mutual exclusivity assumption by young word learners , 2003, Cognitive Psychology.

[5]  P. Kuhl Perception of auditory equivalence classes for speech in early infancy , 1983 .

[6]  V. Mann,et al.  Development of voice recognition: parallels with face recognition. , 1979, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[7]  M. Mills,et al.  Recognition of mother's voice in early infancy , 1974, Nature.

[8]  Olivier Pascalis,et al.  Mother's face recognition by neonates: A replication and an extension , 1995 .

[9]  Sylvia M. J. Hains,et al.  Faceness or complexity: Evidence from newborn visual tracking of facelike stimuli , 1999 .

[10]  B. Bartholomeus Voice identification by nursery school children. , 1973, Canadian journal of psychology.

[11]  P. Kuhl Speech perception in early infancy: perceptual constancy for spectrally dissimilar vowel categories. , 1979, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[12]  S. Carey,et al.  Developmental changes in the representation of faces. , 1977, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[13]  Leanne Proops,et al.  Cross-modal individual recognition in domestic horses (Equus caballus) extends to familiar humans , 2012, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[14]  I. Bushnell,et al.  Mother's face recognition in newborn infants: learning and memory , 2001 .

[15]  W. Merriman,et al.  The mutual exclusivity bias in children's word learning. , 1989, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[16]  S. Carey The child as word learner , 1978 .

[17]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  Learning Adjectives in the Real World: How Learning Nouns Impedes Learning Adjectives , 2007 .

[18]  A. Slater,et al.  Intermodal perception at birth: newborn infants' memory for arbitrary auditory–visual pairings , 1997 .

[19]  Sumarga H. Suanda,et al.  Intersensory redundancy accelerates preverbal numerical competence , 2008, Cognition.

[20]  G. Diesendruck,et al.  Children's use of syntactic and pragmatic knowledge in the interpretation of novel adjectives. , 2006, Child development.

[21]  G. Diesendruck,et al.  Children's Use of Syntactic and Pragmatic Knowledge in the Interpretation of , 2006 .

[22]  E. Markman,et al.  Children's use of mutual exclusivity to constrain the meanings of words , 1988, Cognitive Psychology.

[23]  Leslie M. Bailey,et al.  Young children and adults use lexical principles to learn New Nouns , 1992 .

[24]  D. Maurer,et al.  Infants' perception of natural and distorted arrangements of a schematic face. , 1981, Child development.

[25]  L. Gogate,et al.  Intersensory Redundancy and 7-Month-Old Infants' Memory for Arbitrary Syllable-Object Relations , 2001 .

[26]  O. Pascalis,et al.  Recognition memory in 3- to 4-day-old human neonates. , 1994, Neuroreport.

[27]  Larissa K. Samuelson,et al.  Fast Mapping but Poor Retention by 24-Month-Old Infants. , 2008, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[28]  Thierry Nazzi,et al.  Unfamiliar voice discrimination for short stimuli in newborns , 2000 .

[29]  T. Bower,et al.  Recognition of familiar faces by newborns , 1992 .

[30]  S. Carey,et al.  Development of face recognition: A maturational component? , 1980 .

[31]  O. Pascalis,et al.  Plasticity of face processing in infancy. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[32]  J. Halberda,et al.  Is this a dax which I see before me? Use of the logical argument disjunctive syllogism supports word-learning in children and adults , 2006, Cognitive Psychology.

[33]  A. Decasper,et al.  Human newborns' perception of male voices: preference, discrimination, and reinforcing value. , 1984, Developmental psychobiology.

[34]  U. Neisser Concepts and Conceptual Development: Ecological and Intellectual Factors in Categorization , 1989 .

[35]  Rochelle S. Newman,et al.  Listen to your mother! The role of talker familiarity in infant streaming , 2004, Cognition.

[36]  E. Markman Constraints on word learning: Speculations about their nature, origins, and domain specificity. , 1992 .

[37]  R. Johnston,et al.  Individual recognition by use of odours in golden hamsters: the nature of individual representations , 2001, Animal Behaviour.

[38]  L. Markson,et al.  Evidence against a dedicated system for word learning in children , 1997, Nature.

[39]  S. Hains,et al.  Effects of Experience on Fetal Voice Recognition , 2003, Psychological science.

[40]  L. Markson,et al.  Children's avoidance of lexical overlap: a pragmatic account. , 2001 .

[41]  E. Spelke,et al.  Intermodal exploration and knowledge in infancy , 1979 .

[42]  D. Maurer,et al.  Discrimination of Strangers by the Three-Month-Old. , 1981 .

[43]  J. Mullennix,et al.  Some consequences of stimulus variability on speech processing by 2-month-old infants , 1992, Cognition.

[44]  L. Cohen,et al.  Concept acquisition in the human infant. , 1979, Child development.

[45]  G. Bower,et al.  Depth of processing pictures of faces and recognition memory , 1974 .

[46]  Sandra R. Waxman,et al.  Principles that are invoked in the acquisition of words, but not facts , 2000, Cognition.

[47]  B. A. Baldwin,et al.  Of Human Bonding: Newborns Prefer Their Mothers' Voices , 1980 .

[48]  R. Lickliter,et al.  Infant Discrimination of Voices: Predictions from the Intersensory Redundancy Hypothesis , 2003 .

[49]  E. Clark On the pragmatics of contrast , 1990, Journal of Child Language.

[50]  G. Diesendruck The principles of conventionality and contrast in word learning: an empirical examination. , 2005, Developmental psychology.

[51]  Kevin G. Munhall,et al.  Something in the way she moves , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[52]  D. Pisoni,et al.  Influence of voice similarity on talker discrimination in children with normal hearing and children with cochlear implants. , 2005, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[53]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  Some Beginnings of Word Comprehension in 6-Month-Olds , 1999 .

[54]  Gordon E. Legge,et al.  Learning unfamiliar voices , 1984 .

[55]  D. Lewkowicz,et al.  Three‐month‐old infants learn arbitrary auditory–visual pairings between voices and faces , 2001 .

[56]  Krista M Wilkinson,et al.  The effect of ‘missing’ information on children's retention of fast-mapped labels , 2003, Journal of Child Language.

[57]  Susan Carey,et al.  Acquiring a Single New Word , 1978 .

[58]  David Reby,et al.  Cross-modal individual recognition in domestic horses (Equus caballus) , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[59]  J. Fagan Infants' recognition of invariant features of faces , 1976 .

[60]  L. Bahrick,et al.  The development of infant learning about specific face-voice relations. , 2005, Developmental psychology.

[61]  Edward H. Cornell,et al.  Infants' Discrimination of Photographs of Faces Following Redundant Presentations. , 1974 .

[62]  J. Werker,et al.  Infants' ability to match dynamic phonetic and gender information in the face and voice. , 2002, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[63]  C. Mervis Child-basic object categories and early lexical development. , 1987 .

[64]  A. G. Goldstein,et al.  RECOGNITION BY CHILDREN OF INVERTED PHOTOGRAPHS OF FACES. , 1963, Child development.

[65]  Jacques Mehler,et al.  Infant Recognition of Mother's Voice , 1978, Perception.

[66]  Terry Kit-fong Au,et al.  Acquiring word meanings via linguistic contrast , 1987 .

[67]  C. Mervis,et al.  Acquisition of the novel name--nameless category (N3C) principle. , 1994, Child development.

[68]  O. Pascalis,et al.  The Other-Race Effect Develops During Infancy , 2007, Psychological science.

[69]  Count Nouns, Adjectives, and Perceptual Properties in Children's Novel Word Interpretations , 1992 .

[70]  E. Vatikiotis-Bateson,et al.  `Putting the Face to the Voice' Matching Identity across Modality , 2003, Current Biology.

[71]  E. Markman,et al.  Children's sensitivity to constraints on word meaning: Taxonomic versus thematic relations , 1984, Cognitive Psychology.

[72]  E. Markman,et al.  The relative strengths of indirect and direct word learning. , 2003, Developmental psychology.

[73]  C. Mervis,et al.  Early object labels: the case for a developmental lexical principles framework , 1994, Journal of Child Language.

[74]  Lori Markson,et al.  Are there principles that apply only to the acquisition of words? A reply to Waxman and Booth , 2001, Cognition.

[75]  E. Bushnell,et al.  Infants' sensitivity to arbitrary pairings of color and taste , 1988 .

[76]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  Children Extend Both Words and Non-Verbal Actions to Novel Exemplars , 2003 .